Odour plumes and odour-mediated flight in insects.

Flying insects often follow odour plumes to find resources. Some insects may employ an 'aim-and-shoot' strategy using mechanoreceptors before flight to determine wind direction. Once airborne, insects must use optomotor anemotaxis to set a course upwind. This mechanism uses a visual appraisal of how wind modifies the insect's path. A straight upwind course yields a front-to-rear image flow directly below the insect. Details of this process in male moths flying to female pheromone have emerged mainly from wind tunnel studies. Loss of the pheromone triggers 'casting', or wide lateral excursions without upwind progress, whereas contact with a plume usually induces a zigzag path upwind. The temporally regular counterturns in casting and zigzagging seem to be generated by a central programme. Brief contact with a filament of odour induces a heading towards upwind, and an optimal rate of encounter promotes a rapid, straight upwind course. Other insects, such as parasitoid wasps seeking a host and tsetse flies seeking a blood meal, seem not to have a temporally regular pattern of counter-turns and often fly straight upwind. The availability of visual cues from the odour source itself, the aerial distribution of odour set by turbulent diffusion, and light and wind levels all influence the success of these manoeuvres.

[1]  T. Baker,et al.  Reiterative responses to single strands of odor promote sustained upwind flight and odor source location by moths. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  G. Gibson,et al.  Visually‐guided, upwind turning behaviour of free‐flying tsetse flies in odour‐laden wind: a wind‐tunnel study , 1989 .

[3]  M. J. Packer,et al.  Orientation of tsetse flies to wind, within and outside host odour plumes in the field , 1991 .

[4]  Effect of the fine‐scale structure of pheromone plumes: pulse frequency modulates activation and upwind flight of almond moth males , 1995 .

[5]  J. Kennedy,et al.  The concepts of olfactory ‘arrestment’ and ‘attraction’ , 1978 .

[6]  R. Cardé,et al.  Flight manoeuvers used by a parasitic wasp to locate host‐infested plant , 1994 .

[7]  C. Schal,et al.  Sex pheromone gland of the female tiger moth Holomelina lamae (Lepidoptera: Arctiidae) , 1991 .

[8]  E. Bursell,et al.  Observations on the orientation of tsetse flies (Glossina pallidipes) to wind‐borne odours , 1984 .

[9]  Q. Paynter,et al.  Rates of progress up odour plumes by tsetse flies: a mark‐release video study of the timing of odour source location by Glossina pallidipes , 1995 .

[10]  E. F. Taschenberg,et al.  Sex pheromone of the moth, Antheraea polyphemus , 1975 .

[11]  W. J. Bell,et al.  Vertical community structure and resource utilization in neotropical forest cockroaches , 1986 .

[12]  S. Foster,et al.  Behavior and Integration , 1995 .

[13]  R. T. CARDÉ,et al.  Learning and the role of host‐specific volatiles during in‐flight host‐finding in the specialist parasitoid Microplitis croceipes , 1991 .

[14]  J. Kennedy Zigzagging and casting as a programmed response to wind‐borne odour: a review , 1983 .

[15]  Coby Schal,et al.  Pheromone puff trajectory and upwind flight of male gypsy moths in a forest , 1987 .

[16]  P. Witzgall,et al.  Direct Measurement of the Flight Behavior of Male Moths to Calling Females and Synthetic Sex Pheromones , 1990 .

[17]  M. J. Packer,et al.  Odour movement, wind direction, and the problem of host‐finding by tsetse flies , 1989 .

[18]  R. Cardé,et al.  Influence of plume structure and pheromone concentration on upwind flight of Cadra cautella males , 1995 .

[19]  John Murtis,et al.  Odor Plumes and How Insects Use Them , 1992 .

[20]  Ring T. Cardé,et al.  Strategies for recontacting a lost pheromone plume: casting and upwind flight in the male gypsy moth , 1994 .

[21]  Y. C. Drost,et al.  Use of learned visual cues during habitat location by Brachymeria intermedia , 1992 .

[22]  R. Cardé,et al.  Fine-scale structure of pheromone plumes modulates upwind orientation of flying moths , 1994, Nature.

[23]  G. Gibson,et al.  Host-seeking behavior and management of tsetse. , 1992, Annual review of entomology.

[24]  W. Roelofs,et al.  Pheromone Components and Active Spaces: What Do Moths Smell and Where Do They Smell It? , 1987, Science.

[25]  Thomas C. Baker,et al.  A non‐anemotactic mechanism used in pheromone source location by flying moths , 1983 .

[26]  W. J. Lewis,et al.  Parasitoid foraging and learning. , 1995 .

[27]  J. Charlwood,et al.  Evidence for a ‘memorized’ home range in Anopheles farauti females from Papua New Guinea , 1988, Medical and veterinary entomology.

[28]  S. R. Farkas,et al.  Chemical Trail-Following by Flying Insects: A Mechanism for Orientation to a Distant Odor Source , 1972, Science.

[29]  Search strategies of fruit flies in steady and shifting winds in the absence of food odours , 1994 .

[30]  The effect of wind‐borne odours on the direction of flight in tsetse flies, Glossina spp. , 1987 .

[31]  G. Gibson,et al.  Flight behaviour of tsetse flies in host odour plumes: the initial response to leaving or entering odour , 1988 .

[32]  Q. Paynter,et al.  Wind speed effects on odour source location by tsetse flies (Glossina) , 1995 .