Scopolamine-Induced Impairment of Delayed Recognition of Abstract Visual Shapes

Since the limbic system, whose involvement in cognitive processes is well documented, constitutes a major central cholinergic area, the effect of cholinergic drugs on cognitive tasks has been studied extensively. In the present study, we used a long-term visual recognition task to evaluate the persistence of the scopolamine-induced anterograde amnesia beyond drug clearance intervals. Following memorization of a list of abstract shapes, subjects were evaluated on recognition performance immediately after encoding, and after a 3-day interval. Administration of scopolamine (0.4–0.8 mg) 70 min prior to encoding induced a significant (8–16%) deficit in delayed recognition performance. In contrast, a scopolamine challenge on delayed recognition following a drug-free encoding did not influence memory performance. In contrast, even at peak levels, scopolamine did not alter immediate recognition, detection or visual discriminative performances. Hence, the presence of scopolamine during the encoding of the shapes induced a significant long-term memory deficit that persisted after scopolamine clearance. Therefore, this paradigm is useful for imaging regional brain activation during impaired recognition without the confounding direct effects of scopolamine on cerebral blood flow or metabolism, two physiological variables underlying the indirect measurement of brain activation.

[1]  J. Sirviö,et al.  The interaction of L-deprenyl and scopolamine on spatial learning/memory in rats , 1993, Journal of neural transmission. Parkinson's disease and dementia section.

[2]  G. Orban,et al.  Blood Flow in Human Anterior Temporal Cortex Decreases with Stimulus Familiarity , 1995, NeuroImage.

[3]  J. Rusted,et al.  Perspectives on cognitive psychopharmacology research. , 1996, Behavioural pharmacology.

[4]  Hidenao Fukuyama,et al.  Scopolamine abolishes cerebral blood flow response to somatosensory stimulation in anesthetized cats: PET study , 1994, Brain Research.

[5]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Noradrenergic and cholinergic interactions in the amygdala and the modulation of memory storage , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[6]  G. Dawson,et al.  The effects of novel cholinesterase inhibitors and selective muscarinic receptor agonists in tests of reference and working memory , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  J. Rusted,et al.  Cholinergic control of cognitive resources. , 1993, Neuropsychobiology.

[8]  G. Orban,et al.  Diazepam decreases performance in a long-term memory task using visual shapes. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[9]  S. Iversen,et al.  Induction of cognitive impairment by scopolamine and noncholinergic agents in rhesus monkeys. , 1991, Life sciences.

[10]  S. Iversen,et al.  Direct comparison of cognitive facilitation by physostigmine and tetrahydroaminoacridine in two primate models , 1990, Neurobiology of Aging.

[11]  Ralph Roskies,et al.  Fourier Descriptors for Plane Closed Curves , 1972, IEEE Transactions on Computers.

[12]  I. Izquierdo,et al.  Memory processing by the limbic system: Role of specific neurotransmitter systems , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  W. J. Jackson,et al.  Scopolamine reversal of nicotine enhanced delayed matching-to-sample performance in monkeys , 1993, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[14]  A. Bond,et al.  The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings , 1974 .

[15]  K O Johnson,et al.  Sensory discrimination: decision process. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Encoding and retrieval of episodic memories: Role of cholinergic and GABAergic modulation in the hippocampus , 1998, Hippocampus.