A Model of Semantics and Corrections in Language Learning
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Dana Angluin,et al. Inference of Reversible Languages , 1982, JACM.
[2] Pierre Dupont,et al. Stochastic Grammatical Inference with Multinomial Tests , 2002, ICGI.
[3] A. M. Schaerlaekens,et al. The two-word sentence in child language development : a study based on evidence provided by Dutch-speaking triplets , 1973 .
[4] Isabelle Tellier,et al. Meaning Helps Learning Syntax , 1998, ICGI.
[5] Jeffrey Mark Siskind,et al. Lexical Acquisition in the Presence of Noise and Homonymy , 1994, AAAI.
[6] José Oncina,et al. Learning Stochastic Regular Grammars by Means of a State Merging Method , 1994, ICGI.
[7] G. Marcus. Negative evidence in language acquisition , 1993, Cognition.
[8] Makoto Kanazawa. Learnable Classes of Categorial Grammars , 1998 .
[9] Brian Scassellati,et al. Using sentence context and implicit contrast to learn sensor-grounded meanings for relational and deictic words: the twig system , 2008 .
[10] E. Mark Gold,et al. Language Identification in the Limit , 1967, Inf. Control..
[11] E. Clark,et al. Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence , 2003, Journal of Child Language.
[12] Eve V. Clark,et al. The Lexicon in Acquisition , 1996 .
[13] R. Treiman,et al. Brown & Hanlon revisited: mothers' sensitivity to ungrammatical forms , 1984, Journal of Child Language.
[14] Eve V. Clark,et al. The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. , 1987 .
[15] Brian Scassellati,et al. Robotic vocabulary building using extension inference and implicit contrast , 2009, Artificial Intelligence.
[16] Colin de la Higuera,et al. Probabilistic DFA Inference using Kullback-Leibler Divergence and Minimality , 2000, ICML.
[17] Alex Pentland,et al. Learning words from sights and sounds: a computational model , 2002, Cogn. Sci..
[18] Catherine L. Harris,et al. The human semantic potential: Spatial language and constrained connectionism , 1997 .
[19] Leonor Becerra-Bonache,et al. Learning Meaning Before Syntax , 2008, ICGI.
[20] L L Travis,et al. Limits on negative information in language input , 1989, Journal of Child Language.
[21] Nick Chater,et al. Distributional Information: A Powerful Cue for Acquiring Syntactic Categories , 1998, Cogn. Sci..
[22] Andreas Stolcke,et al. Miniature Language Acquisition: A Touchstone for Cognitive Science , 2002 .
[23] Alexander Clark,et al. Unsupervised Language Acquisition: Theory and Practice , 2002, ArXiv.
[24] Jerome A. Feldman,et al. When push comes to shove: a computational model of the role of motor control in the acquisition of action verbs , 1997 .
[25] C. Snow,et al. Feedback to first language learners: the role of repetitions and clarification questions , 1986, Journal of Child Language.
[26] Dana H. Ballard,et al. A multimodal learning interface for grounding spoken language in sensory perceptions , 2004, ACM Trans. Appl. Percept..
[27] Francisco Casacuberta,et al. Probabilistic finite-state machines - part II , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence.
[28] Jane C. Hill. A computational model of language acquisition in the two-years-old , 1982 .
[29] Dana Ron,et al. On the learnability and usage of acyclic probabilistic finite automata , 1995, COLT '95.
[30] Yasubumi Sakakibara,et al. Efficient Learning of Context-Free Grammars from Positive Structural Examples , 1992, Inf. Comput..
[31] Terry Regier,et al. The Emergence of Words: Attentional Learning in Form and Meaning , 2005, Cogn. Sci..
[32] Brian Scassellati,et al. A Robot That Uses Existing Vocabulary to Infer Non-Visual Word Meanings from Observation , 2007, AAAI.
[33] J. Siskind. A computational study of cross-situational techniques for learning word-to-meaning mappings , 1996, Cognition.