Synchronising movements with the sounds of a virtual partner enhances partner likeability

Abstract Previous studies have demonstrated that synchronising movements with other people can influence affiliative behaviour towards them. While research has focused on synchronisation with visually observed movement, synchronisation with a partner who is heard may have similar effects. We replicate findings showing that synchronisation can influence ratings of likeability of a partner, but demonstrate that this is possible with virtual interaction, involving a video of a partner. Participants performed instructed synchrony in time to sounds instead of the observable actions of another person. Results show significantly higher ratings of likeability of a partner after moving at the same time as sounds attributed to that partner, compared with moving in between sounds. Objectively quantified synchrony also correlated with ratings of likeability. Belief that sounds were made by another person was manipulated in Experiment 2, and results demonstrate that when sounds are attributed to a computer, ratings of likeability are not affected by moving in or out of time. These findings demonstrate that interaction with sound can be experienced as social interaction in the absence of genuine interpersonal contact, which may help explain why people enjoy engaging with recorded music.

[1]  Joy Ollen,et al.  A criterion-related validity test of selected indicators of musical sophistication using expert ratings , 2006 .

[2]  Kevin A. Pelphrey,et al.  Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders , 2008, Schizophrenia Research.

[3]  Scott S. Wiltermuth Synchrony and destructive obedience , 2012 .

[4]  G. Rizzolatti The mirror neuron system and its function in humans , 2005, Anatomy and Embryology.

[5]  Gregory A. Bryant,et al.  Music and dance as a coalition signaling system , 2003, Human nature.

[6]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  R. Soussignan,et al.  Gaze avoidance in social phobia: objective measure and correlates. , 2010, Behaviour research and therapy.

[8]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Do birds of a feather move together? Group membership and behavioral synchrony , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  C. Keysers,et al.  Empathy and the Somatotopic Auditory Mirror System in Humans , 2006, Current Biology.

[10]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Too late to coordinate: Contextual influences on behavioral synchrony , 2009 .

[11]  S. Koelsch,et al.  Understanding the intentions behind man-made products elicits neural activity in areas dedicated to mental state attribution. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  Scott S. Wiltermuth,et al.  Synchrony and Cooperation , 2009, Psychological science.

[13]  C. Frith,et al.  Follow you, Follow me: Continuous Mutual Prediction and Adaptation in Joint Tapping , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  Z. Eitan,et al.  HOW MUSIC MOVES: Musical Parameters and Listeners' Images of Motion , 2006 .

[15]  J. Kelso,et al.  Social coordination dynamics: Measuring human bonding , 2008, Social neuroscience.

[16]  Piercarlo Valdesolo,et al.  Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. , 2011, Emotion.

[17]  D. Keuning How music moves , 2014 .

[18]  Michael J. Hove,et al.  It's all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation , 2009 .

[19]  M. LaFrance Nonverbal synchrony and rapport: Analysis by the cross-lag panel technique. , 1979 .

[20]  D. Lakens Movement synchrony and perceived entitativity , 2010 .

[21]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The Chameleon Effect as Social Glue: Evidence for the Evolutionary Significance of Nonconscious Mimicry , 2003 .

[22]  C. Keysers,et al.  Synchronized Drumming Enhances Activity in the Caudate and Facilitates Prosocial Commitment - If the Rhythm Comes Easily , 2011, PloS one.

[23]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[24]  T. Paus,et al.  Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  A. Modigliani,et al.  Embarrassment, facework, and eye contact: testing a theory of embarrassment. , 1971, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint drumming: social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[27]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Embodied Grounding: Social, Cognitive, Affective, and Neuroscientific Approaches , 2008 .

[28]  A. Isen,et al.  Success, failure, attention, and reaction to others: The warm glow of success. , 1970 .

[29]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Beyond the Perception-Behavior Link: The Ubiquitous Utility and Motivational Moderators of Nonconscious Mimicry. , 2005 .

[30]  Istvan Molnar-Szakacs,et al.  Being Together in Time: Musical Experience and the Mirror Neuron System , 2009 .

[31]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Philip B. Mitchell,et al.  Oxytocin Enhances the Encoding of Positive Social Memories in Humans , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[33]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[34]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Weaving the fabric of social interaction: Articulating developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience in the domain of motor cognition , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[35]  C. Macrae,et al.  The rhythm of rapport: Interpersonal synchrony and social perception , 2009 .

[36]  C. Kleinke Gaze and eye contact: a research review. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[37]  Aina Puce,et al.  Different categories of living and non-living sound-sources activate distinct cortical networks , 2009, NeuroImage.

[38]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  V. Gallese,et al.  Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  Philip B. Mitchell,et al.  Oxytocin Increases Gaze to the Eye Region of Human Faces , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[41]  Freya Bailes,et al.  Synchronization can influence trust following virtual interaction. , 2013, Experimental psychology.

[42]  Ran R. Hassin,et al.  The New Unconscious , 2006 .

[43]  J. Issartel,et al.  Unintended interpersonal co-ordination: “can we march to the beat of our own drum?” , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.

[44]  J. Kelso,et al.  From the Cover : The phi complex as a neuromarker of human social coordination , 2007 .

[45]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[46]  M. Iacoboni Neural mechanisms of imitation , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[47]  Guy Madison,et al.  On the limits of anisochrony in pulse attribution , 2002, Psychological research.

[48]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Grounding social cognition: Synchronization, coordination, and co-regulation. , 2008 .

[49]  Alexander P. Demos,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : General Rocking to the Beat : Effects of Music and Partner ' s Movements on Spontaneous Interpersonal Coordination , 2011 .