The Type a Behavior Pattern and Immune Reactivity to Brief Stress: Change of Volume of Secretory Immunoglobulin a in Saliva

This article presents findings of a laboratory experiment on the association of the Type A behavior pattern with reactivity of secretory immune functioning to brief stress. 38 female undergraduate students classified as Type A (n = 19) or as Type B (n = 19) on the basis of their scores on the Kwansei Gakuin Type A scale performed a continuous arithmetic task in a situation in which they were exposed to aversive loud noise. Secretory immunoglobulin A (s-IgA) in saliva and autonomic measures (heart rate and frequency of eyeblink) were evaluated before and after the manipulation of stress. The volume of s-IgA at baseline was significantly higher for the Type A group than for the Type B group, suggesting that the former relative to the latter might be chronically higher in mucosal immune functioning. Also, the volume of s-IgA significantly increased after exposure to a brief stress for the Type B group but did not change for the Type A group, a finding which might indicate that the Type A group may have less immune reactivity to a brief stress.

[1]  Evidence that secretory IgA antibody is associated with daily mood. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  A. Stone,et al.  Secretory IgA as a measure of immunocompetence. , 1987, Journal of human stress.

[3]  M. Friedman,et al.  Association of specific overt behavior pattern with blood and cardiovascular findings; blood cholesterol level, blood clotting time, incidence of arcus senilis, and clinical coronary artery disease. , 1959, Journal of the American Medical Association.

[4]  H. Merckelbach,et al.  Eyeblink frequency, rehearsal activity, and sympathetic arousal. , 1990, The International journal of neuroscience.

[5]  T. Patterson,et al.  Chronic Life Stress Alters Sympathetic, Neuroendocrine, and Immune Responsivity to an Acute Psychological Stressor in Humans , 1997, Psychosomatic medicine.

[6]  J. Dimsdale,et al.  Psychologic characteristics associated with acute stressor-induced leukocyte subset redistribution. , 1996, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[7]  Matthew F. Muldoon,et al.  Individual Differences in Cellular Immune Response to Stress , 1991 .

[8]  S. Cohen,et al.  Stress and immunity in humans: a meta‐analytic review. , 1993, Psychosomatic medicine.

[9]  K. A. Caudell,et al.  Neuroendocrine and Immunological Responses of Women to Stress , 1995, Western journal of nursing research.

[10]  J. Jemmott,et al.  Academic stress, social support, and secretory immunoglobulin A. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  S. Maier,et al.  Cytokines for psychologists: implications of bidirectional immune-to-brain communication for understanding behavior, mood, and cognition. , 1998, Psychological review.

[12]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Acute psychological stressors and short-term immune changes: what, why, for whom, and to what extent? , 1992, Psychosomatic medicine.

[13]  K. Matthews,et al.  Sympathetic Reactivity to Acute Stress and Immune Response in Women , 1995, Psychosomatic medicine.

[14]  P. Brandtzaeg,et al.  An enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay for differential quantitation of secretory immunoglobulins of the A and M isotypes in human serum. , 1986, Journal of immunological methods.

[15]  J. Mcghee,et al.  Immunoglobulin A (IgA): molecular and cellular interactions involved in IgA biosynthesis and immune response. , 1987, Advances in immunology.

[16]  B. Houston,et al.  Response to stress, cognitive coping strategies, and the type A behavior pattern. , 1980, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  S. Manuck,et al.  Lymphocyte subset and cellular immune responses to a brief experimental stressor. , 1992, Psychosomatic medicine.