Neurobiological regret and rejoice functions for aversive outcomes

A decision maker may experience regret when a choice he makes results in a more adverse outcome than a different choice would have yielded. Analogously, he may experience rejoice when his choice resulted in better outcomes. We used fMRI to investigate the neural correlates of regret and rejoice where payoffs are in terms of a non-monetary medium. Incentives were created using painful outcomes in the form of mild electrical shocks to the foot and the possibility of avoiding them. We hypothesized that the neural response to a painful outcome resulting from an individual's choice would also reflect the degree of regret as measured by the likelihood that alternative choices would have yielded the same adverse outcome. Similarly, when an individual avoids a potential shock, he would experience a degree of rejoice that correlates with the probability he had of receiving the shock. For example, winning a bet when winning was unlikely, even if the outcome is the same, evokes more rejoice than winning when it was highly probable. Our results suggest that activation of a cortical network, consisting of the medial orbitofrontal cortex, left superior frontal cortex, right angular gyrus, and left thalamus, correlates with the degree of regret. A different network, including the rostral anterior cingulate, left hippocampus, left ventral striatum, and brain stem/midbrain correlated with rejoice. The right inferior orbitofrontal cortex, pre-supplementary motor area, anterior cingulate, and posterior cingulate showed similar patterns of activation with both regret and rejoice, suggesting that these regions may be associated with surprise from the realization of relatively unlikely events. Our results suggest that distinct, but overlapping networks are involved in the experiences of regret and rejoice.

[1]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Predictability Modulates Human Brain Response to Reward , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  M. Zeelenberg Anticipated regret, expected feedback and behavioral decision-making , 1999 .

[3]  M. Kawato,et al.  Different neural correlates of reward expectation and reward expectation error in the putamen and caudate nucleus during stimulus-action-reward association learning. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. , 2001, Emotion.

[5]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Regret and its avoidance: a neuroimaging study of choice behavior , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  David L. Donoho,et al.  De-noising by soft-thresholding , 1995, IEEE Trans. Inf. Theory.

[7]  Roland Peyron,et al.  Role of Operculoinsular Cortices in Human Pain Processing: Converging Evidence from PET, fMRI, Dipole Modeling, and Intracerebral Recordings of Evoked Potentials , 2002, NeuroImage.

[8]  Pam Briggs,et al.  Locked Into Gambling: Anticipatory Regret as a Motivator for Playing the National Lottery , 2004, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[9]  Erkut Y. Ozbay,et al.  Auctions with Anticipated Regret: Theory and Experiment , 2007 .

[10]  J. Gläscher,et al.  Dissociable Systems for Gain- and Loss-Related Value Predictions and Errors of Prediction in the Human Brain , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Where and When the Anterior Cingulate Cortex Modulates Attentional Response: Combined fMRI and ERP Evidence , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  W. Penny,et al.  ANOVAs and SPM , 2003 .

[13]  R. D. Smith,et al.  Is Regret Theory an alternative basis for estimating the value of healthcare interventions? , 1996, Health policy.

[14]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[15]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited , 1995, NeuroImage.

[16]  J. Dyer,et al.  A Generalized Utility Model of Disappointment and Regret Effects on Post-Choice Valuation , 1997 .

[17]  G. Pagnoni,et al.  Neurobiological Substrates of Dread , 2006, Science.

[18]  Thomas J. Ross,et al.  Neuroanatomical dissociation between bottom–up and top–down processes of visuospatial selective attention , 2006, NeuroImage.

[19]  E. Rolls,et al.  Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  V. Mittal,et al.  Regret: A Model of its Antecedents and Consequences in Consumer Decision Making , 2000 .

[21]  Alexander Muermann,et al.  Regret, Portfolio Choice, and Guarantees in Defined Contribution Schemes , 2006 .

[22]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[23]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Temporal Prediction Errors in a Passive Learning Task Activate Human Striatum , 2003, Neuron.

[24]  J. Baron,et al.  Outcome Knowledge, Regret, and Omission Bias , 1995 .

[25]  B. Gold,et al.  Functional Dissociation in Frontal and Striatal Areas for Processing of Positive and Negative Reward Information , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Imaging valuation models in human choice. , 2006, Annual review of neuroscience.

[27]  E. Rolls The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  David M. Messick,et al.  A Reverse Outcome Bias: The Influence of Multiple Reference Points on the Evaluation of Outcomes and Decisions , 1995 .

[29]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  Alexander Muermann,et al.  The Impact of Regret on the Demand for Insurance , 2004 .

[31]  Paul J Laurienti,et al.  The subjective experience of pain: where expectations become reality. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  David E. Bell,et al.  Regret in Decision Making under Uncertainty , 1982, Oper. Res..

[33]  Kevin McCabe,et al.  Neural signature of fictive learning signals in a sequential investment task , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[35]  R. Sugden,et al.  Regret Theory: An alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty Review of Economic Studies , 1982 .

[36]  A. Craig,et al.  Pain mechanisms: labeled lines versus convergence in central processing. , 2003, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  N. Roese Counterfactual thinking. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[38]  Todd B. Parrish,et al.  The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention , 2003, NeuroImage.

[39]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  How Many Subjects Constitute a Study? , 1999, NeuroImage.

[41]  David Kelsey,et al.  Regret and disappointment in taxpayer reporting decisions: An experimental study , 1991 .

[42]  Henry B. Tippie Applications of Regret Theory to Asset Pricing , 2002 .

[43]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Roles of Ventral and Dorsal Striatum in Instrumental Conditioning , 2004, Science.

[44]  Masato Inoue,et al.  Neurons in the macaque orbitofrontal cortex code relative preference of both rewarding and aversive outcomes , 2007, Neuroscience Research.

[45]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. , 1999, Science.

[46]  Etienne Mullet,et al.  Avoidance of Anticipated Regret: The Ordering of Prostate-Specific Antigen Tests , 2004, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[47]  B. Mellers Choice and the relative pleasure of consequences. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[48]  N. Yeung,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002 .

[49]  David E. Bell,et al.  Risk Premiums for Decision Regret , 1983 .

[50]  J. Brooks,et al.  REVIEW: From nociception to pain perception: imaging the spinal and supraspinal pathways , 2005, Journal of anatomy.

[51]  Sabine Kastner,et al.  Visual attention as a multilevel selection process , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[52]  Robert Sugden,et al.  Disappointment and Dynamic Consistency in Choice under Uncertainty , 1986 .

[53]  R. Sugden,et al.  Some implications of a more general form of regret theory , 1987 .

[54]  N. Roese,et al.  What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking , 1995 .

[55]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Temporal Difference Models and Reward-Related Learning in the Human Brain , 2003, Neuron.

[56]  P. Montague,et al.  Activity in human ventral striatum locked to errors of reward prediction , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[57]  Alan Schwartz,et al.  Avoiding Future Regret in Purchase-Timing Decisions , 2001 .

[58]  M. Zeelenberg,et al.  Consequences of Regret Aversion: Effects of Expected Feedback on Risky Decision Making , 1996 .

[59]  Soichiro Nomura,et al.  Autonomic arousal in cognitive conflict resolution , 2007, Autonomic Neuroscience.

[60]  Thomas Gilovich,et al.  Varieties of regret: A debate and partial resolution. , 1998 .

[61]  C. Carter,et al.  Outcome representations, counterfactual comparisons and the human orbitofrontal cortex: implications for neuroimaging studies of decision-making. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[62]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Involvement of the Orbitofrontal Cortex in the Experience of Regret , 2004, Science.