Babytwins Study Sweden (BATSS): A multi-method infant twin study of genetic and environmental factors influencing infant brain and behavioral development

Twin studies can help us understand the relative contributions of genes and environment to phenotypic trait variation including attentional and brain activation measures. In terms of applying methodologies like electroencephalography (EEG) and eye tracking, which are key methods in developmental neuroscience, infant twin studies are almost non-existent. Here we describe the Babytwins Study Sweden (BATSS), a multi-method longitudinal twin study of 177 MZ and 134 DZ twin pairs (i.e. 622 individual infants) covering the 5 - 36 month time period. The study includes EEG, eye tracking and genetics, together with more traditional measures based on in-person testing, direct observation and questionnaires. The results show that interest in participation in research among twin parents is high, despite the comprehensive protocol. DNA analysis from saliva samples was possible in virtually all participants, allowing for both zygosity confirmation and polygenic score analyses. Combining a longitudinal twin design with advanced technologies in developmental cognitive neuroscience and genomics, BATSS represents a new approach in infancy research, which we hope to have impact across multiple disciplines in the coming years.

[1]  Hannes P. Eggertsson,et al.  Differences between germline genomes of monozygotic twins , 2021, Nature Genetics.

[2]  S. Bölte,et al.  Parent-child interaction during the first year of life in infants at elevated likelihood of autism spectrum disorder. , 2020, Infant behavior & development.

[3]  S. Ripke,et al.  Mapping genomic loci prioritises genes and implicates synaptic biology in schizophrenia , 2020, medRxiv.

[4]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Look duration at the face as a developmental endophenotype: elucidating pathways to autism and ADHD , 2020, Development and Psychopathology.

[5]  S. Bölte,et al.  Atypical Topographical Organization of Global Form and Motion Processing in 5-Month-Old Infants at Risk for Autism , 2020, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[6]  C. Gillberg,et al.  Etiology of Autism Spectrum Disorders and Autistic Traits Over Time. , 2020, JAMA psychiatry.

[7]  J. Gilmore,et al.  Individual identification and individual variability analysis based on cortical folding features in developing infant singletons and twins , 2020, Human brain mapping.

[8]  V. Diwadkar,et al.  Twin MRI studies on genetic and environmental determinants of brain morphology and function in the early lifespan , 2020, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[9]  N. Wray,et al.  3GENOME-WIDE META-ANALYSIS OF DEPRESSION , 2019, European Neuropsychopharmacology.

[10]  P. Lichtenstein,et al.  The Swedish Twin Registry: Content and Management as a Research Infrastructure , 2019, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[11]  G. Gredebäck,et al.  Play enhances visual form perception in infancy–an active training study , 2019, Developmental science.

[12]  J. Buitelaar,et al.  Eurosibs: Towards robust measurement of infant neurocognitive predictors of autism across Europe , 2019, Infant behavior & development.

[13]  A. Klin,et al.  Behavioral predictors of autism recurrence are genetically independent and influence social reciprocity: evidence that polygenic ASD risk is mediated by separable elements of developmental liability , 2019, Translational Psychiatry.

[14]  H. Goldsmith,et al.  Observed Profiles of Infant Temperament: Stability, Heritability, and Associations With Parenting. , 2019, Child development.

[15]  John P. Rice,et al.  Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder , 2019, Nature Genetics.

[16]  R. Marioni,et al.  Genome-wide meta-analysis of depression identifies 102 independent variants and highlights the importance of the prefrontal brain regions , 2018, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  Alicia R. Martin,et al.  Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder , 2018, Nature Genetics.

[18]  T. Ge,et al.  Polygenic prediction via Bayesian regression and continuous shrinkage priors , 2018, bioRxiv.

[19]  Jonathan P. Beauchamp,et al.  Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals , 2018, Nature Genetics.

[20]  Tyrone D. Cannon,et al.  Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence , 2018, Nature Genetics.

[21]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Enhanced pupillary light reflex in infancy is associated with autism diagnosis in toddlerhood , 2018, Nature Communications.

[22]  C. Nelson,et al.  EEG Analytics for Early Detection of Autism Spectrum Disorder: A data-driven approach , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[23]  P. Visscher,et al.  Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for height and body mass index in ∼700,000 individuals of European ancestry , 2018, bioRxiv.

[24]  J. Constantino,et al.  Early Origins of Autism Comorbidity: Neuropsychiatric Traits Correlated in Childhood Are Independent in Infancy , 2018, Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.

[25]  Daniel P. Kennedy,et al.  Genetic Influence on Eye Movements to Complex Scenes at Short Timescales , 2017, Current Biology.

[26]  David M Williams,et al.  The Wiley Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology , 2017 .

[27]  A. Ronald,et al.  The genetic basis of psychological traits in infancy: Implications for understanding the causes of developmental psychopathology , 2017 .

[28]  Pall I. Olason,et al.  SweGen: a whole-genome data resource of genetic variability in a cross-section of the Swedish population , 2017, European Journal of Human Genetics.

[29]  Melissa J. Green,et al.  Genome-wide association study identifies 30 Loci Associated with Bipolar Disorder , 2017, bioRxiv.

[30]  D. Boomsma,et al.  The etiology of autistic traits in preschoolers: a population‐based twin study , 2017, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[31]  A. Klin,et al.  Infant viewing of social scenes is under genetic control and atypical in autism , 2017, Nature.

[32]  S. Bölte,et al.  Fetal and postnatal metal dysregulation in autism , 2017, Nature Communications.

[33]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Disentangling the mechanisms underlying infant fixation durations in scene perception: A computational account , 2017, Vision Research.

[34]  J. Gilmore,et al.  Twin–singleton developmental study of brain white matter anatomy , 2017, Human brain mapping.

[35]  Rachel J. Steiner,et al.  Common and heritable components of white matter microstructure predict cognitive function at 1 and 2 y , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  P. Mcguire,et al.  Association between stressful life events and psychotic experiences in adolescence: evidence for gene–environment correlations , 2016, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[37]  K. Saudino,et al.  The development of inhibitory control in early childhood: A twin study from 2-3 years. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[38]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Genetic specificity of face recognition , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  C. Wijmenga,et al.  Population-specific genotype imputations using minimac or IMPUTE2 , 2015, Nature Protocols.

[40]  P. Visscher,et al.  Nature Genetics Advance Online Publication , 2022 .

[41]  S. Bölte,et al.  Hypersensitive pupillary light reflex in infants at risk for autism , 2015, Molecular Autism.

[42]  Hongtu Zhu,et al.  Intersubject Variability of and Genetic Effects on the Brain's Functional Connectivity during Infancy , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  Jamie Zibulsky,et al.  Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition , 2014 .

[44]  A. Ronald,et al.  “He who sees things grow from the beginning will have the finest view of them” A systematic review of genetic studies on psychological traits in infancy , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[45]  Tony Charman,et al.  The development of face orienting mechanisms in infants at-risk for autism , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[46]  Michel Boivin,et al.  Genetic and Environmental Influences on Daytime and Nighttime Sleep Duration in Early Childhood , 2013, Pediatrics.

[47]  S. Sparrow,et al.  Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition , 2012 .

[48]  Martin Styner,et al.  White Matter Heritability Using Diffusion Tensor Imaging in Neonatal Brains , 2012, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[49]  J. Wardle,et al.  Genetic and Environmental Influences on Infant Sleep , 2012, Pediatrics.

[50]  R. Plomin,et al.  The etiology of variation in language skills changes with development: a longitudinal twin study of language from 2 to 12 years. , 2012, Developmental science.

[51]  Sebastian Lundström,et al.  The Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS) , 2011, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[52]  T. Cole,et al.  A chart to predict adult height from a child's current height , 2011, Annals of human biology.

[53]  Michel Boivin,et al.  Associations between sleep-wake consolidation and language development in early childhood: a longitudinal twin study. , 2011, Sleep.

[54]  C. Fagnani,et al.  Genetic and Environmental Factors Shape Infant Sleep Patterns: A Study of 18-Month-Old Twins , 2011, Pediatrics.

[55]  Martin Styner,et al.  r Human Brain Mapping 000:000–000 (2010) r Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Neonatal Brain Structure: A Twin Study* , 2022 .

[56]  K. Saudino,et al.  Wait For It! A Twin Study of Inhibitory Control in Early Childhood , 2010, Behavior genetics.

[57]  N. Ilott,et al.  A genetic study of ADHD and activity level in infancy , 2010, Genes, brain, and behavior.

[58]  M. Gernsbacher,et al.  Genetic variance for autism screening items in an unselected sample of toddler-age twins. , 2010, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[59]  A. Ronald,et al.  Exploring the Relationship Between Autistic-Like Traits and ADHD Behaviors in Early Childhood: Findings from a Community Twin Study of 2-Year-Olds , 2010, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[60]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Eye Tracking in Infancy Research , 2009, Developmental neuropsychology.

[61]  P. Donnelly,et al.  A Flexible and Accurate Genotype Imputation Method for the Next Generation of Genome-Wide Association Studies , 2009, PLoS genetics.

[62]  Jodi A Mindell,et al.  Sleep and sleep ecology in the first 3 years: a web‐based study , 2009, Journal of sleep research.

[63]  John O. Willis,et al.  Mullen Scales of Early Learning: Ags Edition , 2008 .

[64]  R. Plomin,et al.  Why do preschool language abilities correlate with later reading? A twin study. , 2008, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[65]  Glenn I. Roisman,et al.  A behavior-genetic study of parenting quality, infant attachment security, and their covariation in a nationally representative sample. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[66]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Q-CHAT (Quantitative CHecklist for Autism in Toddlers): A Normally Distributed Quantitative Measure of Autistic Traits at 18–24 Months of Age: Preliminary Report , 2008, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[67]  H. McConachie,et al.  Repetitive behaviours in typically developing 2-year-olds. , 2007, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[68]  C. Lindgren,et al.  Large-Scale Zygosity Testing Using Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms , 2007, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[69]  Nicholas G Martin,et al.  Common and specific genetic influences on EEG power bands delta, theta, alpha, and beta , 2007, Biological Psychology.

[70]  P. Bolton,et al.  Origins of individual differences in imitation: links with language, pretend play, and socially insightful behavior in two-year-old twins. , 2007, Child development.

[71]  P. Fonagy,et al.  In search of shared and nonshared environmental factors in security of attachment: a behavior-genetic study of the association between sensitivity and attachment security. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[72]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: the early childhood behavior questionnaire. , 2006, Infant behavior & development.

[73]  H. Goldsmith,et al.  A Population-Based Twin Study of Parentally Reported Tactile and Auditory Defensiveness in Young Children , 2006, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[74]  K. Saudino Behavioral Genetics and Child Temperament , 2005, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.

[75]  G. Tell,et al.  Size at Birth and Gestational Age as Predictors of Adult Height and Weight , 2005, Epidemiology.

[76]  C. Lord,et al.  Early Indicators of Autism Spectrum Disorders in the Second Year of Life , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[77]  S. Hunnius,et al.  Developmental Changes in Visual Scanning of Dynamic Faces and Abstract Stimuli in Infants: A Longitudinal Study. , 2004, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[78]  M. Bakermans-Kranenburg,et al.  The importance of shared environment in infant-father attachment: a behavioral genetic study of the attachment q-sort. , 2004, Journal of family psychology : JFP : journal of the Division of Family Psychology of the American Psychological Association.

[79]  H. Goldsmith,et al.  Genetic, environmental, and gender effects on individual differences in toddler expressive language. , 2004, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[80]  Toshihiro Tanaka The International HapMap Project , 2003, Nature.

[81]  P. Fonagy,et al.  The importance of shared environment in mother-infant attachment security: a behavioral genetic study. , 2003, Child development.

[82]  I. Gottesman,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS MODIFIES HERITABILITY OF IQ , 2022 .

[83]  Elena V Orekhova,et al.  Heritability and "environmentability" of electroencephalogram in infants: the twin study. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[84]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Studying infant temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire , 2003 .

[85]  R. Plomin,et al.  Hyperactivity in preschool children is highly heritable. , 2001, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[86]  S. Matthey,et al.  The Sleep and Settle Questionnaire for parents of infants: Psychometric properties , 2001, Journal of paediatrics and child health.

[87]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning Autism, Malesand Females, Scientists and Mathematicians , 2001, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[88]  J. Owens,et al.  The Children's Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ): psychometric properties of a survey instrument for school-aged children. , 2000, Sleep.

[89]  H. Meltzer,et al.  Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. , 2000, The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science.

[90]  R. Plomin,et al.  Lexical and grammatical development: a behavioural genetic perspective , 2000, Journal of Child Language.

[91]  K. Deater-Deckard,et al.  Parent-child mutuality in early childhood: two behavioral genetic studies. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[92]  R. Plomin,et al.  Sex differences in early verbal and non‐verbal cognitive development , 2000 .

[93]  J. Campos,et al.  Genetic analyses of focal aspects of infant temperament. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[94]  R. Plomin,et al.  Exploring the genetic and environmental etiology of high general cognitive ability in fourteen- to thirty-six-month-old twins. , 1998, Child development.

[95]  R. Plomin,et al.  Exploring the genetic etiology of low general cognitive ability from 14 to 36 months. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[96]  L. DiLalla,et al.  Differential maternal treatment of infant twins: Effects on infant behaviors , 1996, Behavior genetics.

[97]  Dante Cicchetti,et al.  Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology , 1996, Development and Psychopathology.

[98]  D I Boomsma,et al.  Genetic architecture of EEG power spectra in early life. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[99]  J. Reznick,et al.  A Developmental-Genetic Analysis of Continuity and Change in the Bayley Mental Development Index from 14 to 24 Months: The MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study , 1994 .

[100]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Components of Visual Orienting in Early Infancy: Contingency Learning, Anticipatory Looking, and Disengaging , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[101]  M. Rothbart Measurement of Temperament in Infancy , 1981 .

[102]  A. Matheny Bayley's infant behavior record: behavioral components and twin analyses. , 1980, Child development.

[103]  S. Counce The Strategy of the Genes , 1958, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[104]  H. Goldsmith,et al.  A longitudinal analysis of anger and inhibitory control in twins from 12 to 36 months of age. , 2011, Developmental science.

[105]  H. Goldsmith,et al.  The Genetics of Childhood Temperament , 2009 .

[106]  G. Gottlieb Probabilistic epigenesis. , 2007, Developmental science.

[107]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Development genetic analysis of general cognitive ability from 1 to 12 years in a sample of adoptees, biological siblings, and twins , 2003 .

[108]  Scientific International Standard Classification of Education, ISCED 1997 , 2003 .

[109]  J. Ganger Genes and environment in language acquisition : a study of early vocabulary and syntactic development in twins , 1998 .

[110]  J. Kagan,et al.  Genetic change and continuity from fourteen to twenty months , 1993 .