Temporal and spatial coupling of point of gaze and hand movements in aiming.

Temporal and spatial coordination of both point of gaze (PG) and hand kinematics in a speeded aiming task toward an eccentrically positioned visual target were examined with the Optotrak 3D movement analysis system in tandem with the ASL head-mounted eye tracker. Subjects (N = 10) moved eyes, head, hand, and trunk freely. On the majority of trials, the PG pattern was a large initial saccade that undershot the target slightly, then 1 or more smaller corrective saccades to reach the target. The hand exhibited a similar pattern of first undershooting the target and then making small corrective movements. Previously (W. F. Helsen, J. L. Starkes, & M. J. Buekers, 1997), the ratio of PG and total hand response time (50%) was found to be an invariant feature of the movement. In line with those results, a striking temporal coupling was found between completion of the primary eye saccade and time to peak acceleration for the limb. Spatially, peak hand velocity coincided with completion of 50% of total movement distance. Those findings support a 2-component model of limb control.

[1]  James L. Lyons,et al.  The utilization of visual information in the control of rapid sequential aiming movements. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[2]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Sampling frequency and the study of eye-hand coordination in aiming , 1998 .

[3]  Janet L. Starkes,et al.  Effects of Target Eccentricity on Temporal Costs of Point of Gaze and the Hand in Aiming , 1997 .

[4]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Rescaling an acquired discrete aiming movement: Specific or general motor learning? , 1997 .

[5]  D Elliott,et al.  Intermittent Vision and Discrete Manual Aiming , 1995, Perceptual and motor skills.

[6]  Réjean Plamondon,et al.  A kinematic theory of rapid human movements , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[7]  Réjean Plamondon,et al.  A kinematic theory of rapid human movements , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[8]  D Elliott,et al.  Optimizing the use of Vision in Manual Aiming: The Role of Practice , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[9]  H Carnahan,et al.  Hand, eye, and head coordination while pointing to perturbed targets. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[10]  E. Komilis,et al.  Error processing in pointing at randomly feedback-induced double-step stimuli. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[11]  D. Elliott,et al.  Visual regulation of manual aiming , 1993 .

[12]  D. Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the regulation of visually guided aiming. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming , 1991 .

[14]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Utilization of Visual Feedback Information during Rapid Pointing Movements , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[15]  Lorraine G. Kisselburgh,et al.  Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  E. R. Crossman,et al.  Feedback Control of Hand-Movement and Fitts' Law , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[17]  L. Carlton Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Daniel W. Repperger,et al.  Fitts' law and the microstructure of rapid discrete movements. , 1980 .

[19]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[20]  D. Chaffin,et al.  An investigation of fitts' law using a wide range of movement amplitudes. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  L. Young,et al.  Survey of eye movement recording methods , 1975 .

[22]  W D Beggs,et al.  The accuracy of aiming at a target. Some further evidence for a theory of intermittent control. , 1972, Acta psychologica.

[23]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. , 1969, Vision research.

[24]  J. Brebner Continuing and reversing the direction of responding movements: some exceptions to the so-called "psychological refractory period". , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[25]  M. Posner,et al.  Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  P. Fitts,et al.  INFORMATION CAPACITY OF DISCRETE MOTOR RESPONSES. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[27]  A. T. Slater-Hammel,et al.  Reliability, Accuracy, and Refractoriness of a Transit Reaction , 1960 .

[28]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[29]  Les G. Carlton,et al.  Chapter 1 Visual Processing Time and the Control of Movement , 1992 .

[30]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[31]  L. Stark,et al.  The trajectories of saccadic eye movements. , 1979, Scientific American.

[32]  G. Fullerton Psychology and physiology. , 1896 .