A Self-Organized Internal Models Architecture for Coding Sensory–Motor Schemes

Cognitive robotics research draws inspiration from theories and models on cognition, as conceived by neuroscience or cognitive psychology, to investigate biologically plausible computational models in artificial agents. In this field, the theoretical framework of Grounded Cognition provides epistemological and methodological grounds for the computational modeling of cognition. It has been stressed in the literature that \textit{simulation}, \textit{prediction}, and \textit{multi-modal integration} are key aspects of cognition and that computational architectures capable of putting them into play in a biologically plausible way are a necessity. Research in this direction has brought extensive empirical evidence suggesting that \textit{Internal Models} are suitable mechanisms for sensory-motor integration. However, current Internal Models architectures show several drawbacks, mainly due to the lack of a unified substrate allowing for a true sensory-motor integration space, enabling flexible and scalable ways to model cognition under the embodiment hypothesis constraints. We propose the Self-Organized Internal Models Architecture (SOIMA), a computational cognitive architecture coded by means of a network of self-organized maps, implementing coupled internal models that allow modeling multi-modal sensory-motor schemes. Our approach addresses integrally the issues of current implementations of Internal Models. We discuss the design and features of the architecture, and provide empirical results on a humanoid robot that demonstrate the benefits and potentialities of the SOIMA concept for studying cognition in artificial agents.

[1]  Rolf Pfeifer,et al.  Understanding intelligence , 2020, Inequality by Design.

[2]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal Teacher , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[3]  Ping Li,et al.  Dynamic Self-Organization and Early Lexical Development in Children , 2007, Cogn. Sci..

[4]  Angelo Cangelosi,et al.  The Mechanics of Embodiment: A Dialog on Embodiment and Computational Modeling , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[5]  S B Udin,et al.  Formation of topographic maps. , 1988, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Why can't you tickle yourself? , 2000, Neuroreport.

[7]  Yiannis Demiris,et al.  Hierarchical attentive multiple models for execution and recognition of actions , 2006, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[8]  Alejandro Hernández Arieta,et al.  Body Schema in Robotics: A Review , 2010, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.

[9]  Anthony M. Dearden Developmental learning of internal models for robotics , 2008 .

[10]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[11]  J. Knott The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory , 1951 .

[12]  K. Plunkett,et al.  A neurocomputational account of taxonomic responding and fast mapping in early word learning. , 2010, Psychological review.

[13]  Bruno Lara,et al.  Internal Simulations for Behaviour Selection and Recognition , 2012, HBU.

[14]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach , 2003, Current Biology.

[15]  Peter Ford Dominey,et al.  Multi-modal convergence maps: from body schema and self-representation to mental imagery , 2013, Adapt. Behav..

[16]  Rachel Morgan,et al.  Two functional orientations of self‐exploration in infancy , 1998 .

[17]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[18]  Teuvo Kohonen,et al.  The self-organizing map , 1990 .

[19]  A. Damasio,et al.  Convergence and divergence in a neural architecture for recognition and memory , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[20]  P. Rochat Self-perception and action in infancy , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  L. Munari How the body shapes the way we think — a new view of intelligence , 2009 .

[22]  Bruno Lara,et al.  Modelado de un Sistema de Neuronas Espejo en un Agente Autónomo Artificial , 2014 .

[23]  Bruno Lara,et al.  Coupled inverse-forward models for action execution leading to tool-use in a humanoid robot , 2012, 2012 7th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[24]  Jianhua Cang,et al.  Developmental mechanisms of topographic map formation and alignment. , 2013, Annual review of neuroscience.

[25]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Perspectives and problems in motor learning , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  Lawrence Stark,et al.  Glissades—eye movements generated by mismatched components of the saccadic motoneuronal control signal , 1975 .

[27]  Mark Dunn,et al.  Online adaptation of gaze fixation for a stereo-vergence system with foveated vision , 2009, 2009 International Conference on Advanced Robotics.

[28]  Heiko Hoffmann,et al.  Grasping of extrafoveal targets: A robotic model , 2011 .

[29]  Lawrence W Barsalou,et al.  Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[30]  Wolfram Schenck,et al.  Solving the correspondence problem in stereo vision by internal simulation , 2013, Adapt. Behav..

[31]  T. Ziemke,et al.  Making sense of embodiment : simulation theories and the sharing of neural circuitry , 2004 .

[32]  Michael J. Spivey,et al.  Computational Grounded Cognition: a new alliance between grounded cognition and computational modeling , 2013, Front. Psychology.

[33]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Thinking With Your Body: Modelling Spatial Biases in Categorization Using a Real Humanoid Robot , 2010 .

[34]  Juyang Weng,et al.  Top–Down Connections in Self-Organizing Hebbian Networks: Topographic Class Grouping , 2010, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.

[35]  Rick Grush,et al.  The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[36]  Giulio Sandini,et al.  Developmental robotics: a survey , 2003, Connect. Sci..

[37]  Wolfram Schenck,et al.  Bootstrapping Cognition from Behavior - A Computerized Thought Experiment , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[38]  Jon H Kaas,et al.  Topographic Maps are Fundamental to Sensory Processing , 1997, Brain Research Bulletin.

[39]  Angelo Cangelosi,et al.  Epigenetic Robotics Architecture (ERA) , 2010, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.

[40]  Antonio R. Damasio,et al.  The Brain Binds Entities and Events by Multiregional Activation from Convergence Zones , 1989, Neural Computation.

[41]  Margaret Wilson,et al.  Six views of embodied cognition , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[42]  Bruno Lara,et al.  Is that me? Sensorimotor learning and self-other distinction in robotics , 2013, 2013 8th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[43]  G. Pezzulo,et al.  Action simulation in the human brain: Twelve questions , 2013 .

[44]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[45]  Wolfram Schenck,et al.  Adaptive internal models for motor control and visual prediction , 2008 .

[46]  M. Asada,et al.  Visual attention by saliency leads cross-modal body representation , 2008, 2008 7th IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning.

[47]  Bruno Lara,et al.  Prediction of Undesired Situations Based on Multi-Modal Representations , 2007, Electronics, Robotics and Automotive Mechanics Conference (CERMA'06).

[48]  A. Maravita,et al.  Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[49]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[50]  Eduardo Miranda,et al.  Modelling the Development of Mirror Neurons for Auditory-Motor Integration , 2002 .

[51]  R. Leigh,et al.  The neurology of eye movements , 2006 .