Proprioception plays a different role for sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions.

If proprioceptive feedback is degraded by agonist-antagonist muscle vibration, then adaptation to rotated vision remains intact while adaptation to a velocity-dependent force field worsens. Here we evaluate whether this differential effect of vibration is related to the physical nature of the distortion - visual versus mechanical - or to their kinematic coupling to the subjects' hand - velocity versus position dependent. Subjects adapted to a velocity-dependent visual distortion, to a position-dependent force, or to a velocity-dependent force; one half of the subjects adapted with, and the other half without agonist-antagonist vibration at the wrist, elbow, and shoulder. We found, as before, that vibration slowed down adaptation to a velocity-dependent force. However, vibration did not modify adaptation to the other two distortions, nor did it influence the aftereffects of any distortion. From this we conclude that intact proprioception supports strategic compensatory processes when proprioceptive signals agree with visual ones, and provide relevant (dynamic) information not available to the visual system.

[1]  D. Willingham,et al.  Deficit in learning of a motor skill requiring strategy, but not of perceptuomotor recalibration, with aging. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[2]  Dottie M. Clower,et al.  Selective use of perceptual recalibration versus visuomotor skill acquisition. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. , 1997, Science.

[4]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  J Blouin,et al.  Adaptation in visuomanual tracking depends on intact proprioception. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[6]  R. Miall,et al.  The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  J. Roll,et al.  Alteration of proprioceptive messages induced by tendon vibration in man: a microneurographic study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Nicolas Vuillerme,et al.  Postural sway under muscle vibration and muscle fatigue in humans , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[9]  E. Taub,et al.  Use of Sensory Recombination and Somatosensory Deafferentation Techniques in the Investigation of Sensory-Motor Integration , 1974, Perception.

[10]  C. Bard,et al.  Contribution of proprioception for calibrating and updating the motor space. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[11]  B. Stein,et al.  The Merging of the Senses , 1993 .

[12]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual capture produced by prism spectacles , 1965 .

[13]  J. C. Gilhodes,et al.  Perceptual and motor effects of agonist-antagonist muscle vibration in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Is sensorimotor adaptation to position- and velocity-dependent visual distortions based on distinct adaptive processes? , 2010, Human movement science.

[15]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  Force-field adaptation without proprioception: Can vision be used to model limb dynamics? , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Interaction of visual and proprioceptive feedback during adaptation of human reaching movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  D. Timmann,et al.  Visuomotor adaptive improvement and aftereffects are impaired differentially following cerebellar lesions in SCA and PICA territory , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  Wrist vibration affects the production of finely graded forces. , 2005, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[19]  J R Lackner,et al.  Visual Tracking of Active and Passive Movements of the Hand , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[20]  N Teasdale,et al.  Mirror drawing in a deafferented patient and normal subjects , 1992, Neurology.

[21]  C. Bourdin,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation to inertial forces in a multi-force environment does not depend on the number of targets: Indirect validation of the altered-proprioception hypothesis , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[22]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Components of sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly subjects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  The contribution of proprioceptive feedback to sensorimotor adaptation , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Otmar Bock,et al.  A method to reversibly degrade proprioceptive feedback in research on human motor control , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[26]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  R. Daroff,et al.  Clinical Neurology for Psychiatrists , 1995, Neurology.

[28]  G. M. Gauthier,et al.  Weight estimation in a "deafferented" man and in control subjects: are judgements influenced by peripheral or central signals? , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Kinematics and Dynamics Are Not Represented Independently in Motor Working Memory: Evidence from an Interference Study , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.