Probing the human stereoscopic system with reverse correlation

Our two eyes obtain slightly different views of the world. The resulting differences in the two retinal images, called binocular disparities, provide us with a stereoscopic sense of depth. The primary visual cortex (V1) contains neurons that are selective for the disparity of individual elements in an image, but this information must be further analysed to complete the stereoscopic process. Here we apply the psychophysical technique of reverse correlation to investigate disparity processing in human vision. Observers viewed binocular random-dot patterns, with ‘signal’ dots in a specific depth plane plus ‘noise’ dots with randomly assigned disparities. By examining the correlation between the observers' ability to detect the plane and the particular sample of ‘noise’ disparities presented on each trial, we revealed detection ‘filters’, whose disparity selectivity was remarkably similar to that of individual neurons in monkey V1. Moreover, if the noise dots were of opposite contrast in the two eyes, the tuning inverted, just like the response patterns of V1 neurons. Reverse correlation appears to probe disparity processing at the earliest stages of binocular combination, prior to the generation of a full stereoscopic depth percept.

[1]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  G. Ermentrout,et al.  Phase transition and other phenomena in chains of coupled oscilators , 1990 .

[3]  D. Leach,et al.  Gait fatigue in the racing thoroughbred [Horses, factors for certain injuries] , 1979 .

[4]  B. Julesz Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1971 .

[5]  R. Mann,et al.  Comparative electromyography of the lower extremity in jogging, running, and sprinting , 1986, The American journal of sports medicine.

[6]  D Marr,et al.  A computational theory of human stereo vision. , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[7]  S. T. Buckland,et al.  An Introduction to the Bootstrap. , 1994 .

[8]  B. G. Cumming,et al.  Responses of primary visual cortical neurons to binocular disparity without depth perception , 1997, Nature.

[9]  N. R. Deuel,et al.  Laterality in the gallop gait of horses. , 1987, Journal of biomechanics.

[10]  Allen I. Selverston,et al.  Are central pattern generators understandable? , 1980, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[11]  G. Ermentrout,et al.  Symmetry and phaselocking in chains of weakly coupled oscillators , 1986 .

[12]  I. Stewart,et al.  Coupled nonlinear oscillators and the symmetries of animal gaits , 1993 .

[13]  I. Ohzawa,et al.  Stereoscopic depth discrimination in the visual cortex: neurons ideally suited as disparity detectors. , 1990, Science.

[14]  Alexander I. Cogan,et al.  Depth in anticorrelated stereograms: Effects of spatial density and interocular delay , 1993, Vision Research.

[15]  M Hildebrand,et al.  Symmetrical gaits of horses. , 1965, Science.

[16]  Lawrence K. Cormack,et al.  Disparity tuning in mechanisms of human stereopsis , 1992, Vision Research.

[17]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[18]  A J Parker,et al.  Binocular disparity processing with opposite-contrast stimuli. , 1995 .

[19]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1967, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  Charles Wheatstone On some remarkable and hitherto unobserved phenomena of binocular vision. , 1962 .

[21]  I. Stewart,et al.  Symmetry-breaking bifurcation: A possible mechanism for 2:1 frequency-locking in animal locomotion , 1992, Journal of mathematical biology.

[22]  R Blake,et al.  Binocular Disparity Processing with Opposite-Contrast Stimuli , 1995, Perception.

[23]  S. Grillner Locomotion in vertebrates: central mechanisms and reflex interaction. , 1975, Physiological reviews.

[24]  Ian Stewart,et al.  A modular network for legged locomotion , 1998 .

[25]  David J. Fleet,et al.  Neural encoding of binocular disparity: Energy models, position shifts and phase shifts , 1996, Vision Research.

[26]  Bernice E. Rogowitz,et al.  Human Vision and Electronic Imaging XVIII , 1998 .

[27]  G. Ermentrout,et al.  Coupled oscillators and the design of central pattern generators , 1988 .

[28]  M. Hildebrand The quadrupedal gaits of vertebrates , 1989 .

[29]  C. Wheatstone XVIII. Contributions to the physiology of vision. —Part the first. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision , 1962, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.

[30]  R. Blickhan,et al.  Leg design in hexapedal runners. , 1991, The Journal of experimental biology.

[31]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Vergence eye movements in response to binocular disparity without depth perception , 1997, Nature.

[32]  C. Schor,et al.  Binocular sensory fusion is limited by spatial resolution , 1984, Vision Research.

[33]  M. Golubitsky,et al.  Models of central pattern generators for quadruped locomotion II. Secondary gaits , 2001, Journal of mathematical biology.

[34]  G. Poggio,et al.  Binocular interaction and depth sensitivity in striate and prestriate cortex of behaving rhesus monkey. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  B G Cumming,et al.  Disparity Detection in Anticorrelated Stereograms , 1998, Perception.

[36]  G Schöner,et al.  A synergetic theory of quadrupedal gaits and gait transitions. , 1990, Journal of theoretical biology.

[37]  L K Cormack,et al.  Disparity-tuned channels of the human visual system , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[38]  Albert J. Ahumada,et al.  Technique to extract relevant image features for visual tasks , 1998, Electronic Imaging.

[39]  M. L. Shik,et al.  Neurophysiology of locomotor automatism. , 1976, Physiological reviews.

[40]  F. Delcomyn Neural basis of rhythmic behavior in animals. , 1980, Science.