A Formal, Computational Theory of Multiple-goal Pursuit: Integrating Goal-choice and Goal-striving Processes We Thank Allison Tenbrink for Helping to Classify the Data, Sheng Liu for Helping with the Modeling, And

Understanding the processes involved when pursuing multiple goals over time is a central question for motivational theorists. A dynamic, computational model integrating theories of goal striving and goal choice is presented to account for data emerging from Schmidt and DeShon's (2007) multiple-goal-pursuit protocol. The simulated results match the results reported in their study, including the finding that relative discrepancy from the goals positively predicted resource allocation early on but negatively predicted it toward the end of the session. Variance in parameters in the model also accounted for individual differences found in the data. Discussion focuses on the theoretical contribution of formally integrating elements of self-regulation theories, further empirical work needed to test the model, and further theoretical work needed to continue the integration process exemplified here.

[1]  E. Higgins,et al.  Beyond pleasure and pain. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[2]  M. Smith Field Theory in Social Science: Selected Theoretical Papers. , 1951 .

[3]  C. L. Hull The problem of intervening variables in molar behavior theory. , 1943 .

[4]  C. Carver,et al.  Origins and Functions of Positive and Negative Affect: A Control-Process View. , 1990 .

[5]  J. B. Vancouver,et al.  The depth of history and explanation as benefit and bane for psychological control theories. , 2005, The Journal of applied psychology.

[6]  Michael E. Hyland,et al.  Motivational control theory: An integrative framework. , 1988 .

[7]  Daniel R. Ilgen,et al.  Computational Modeling of Behavior in Organizations: The Third Scientific Discipline , 2000 .

[8]  Noelle M. Nelson,et al.  Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: a limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  M. Ford Motivating Humans: Goals, Emotions, and Personal Agency Beliefs , 1992 .

[10]  G. Northcraft,et al.  Robbing Peter to pay Paul: Feedback environments and enacted priorities in response to competing task demands , 2003 .

[11]  Yitzhak Fried,et al.  Enriching Goal-Setting Theory with Time: An Integrated Approach , 2004 .

[12]  P. Hanges,et al.  Integrating Neural Networks into Decision-Making and Motivational Theory: Rethinking VIE Theory * , 2003 .

[13]  I. J. Myung,et al.  The Importance of Complexity in Model Selection. , 2000, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[14]  R. Kanfer Motivation theory and industrial and organizational psychology. , 1990 .

[15]  W. T. Powers Behavior, the control of perception , 1973 .

[16]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  A theory of goal systems. , 2002 .

[17]  Rick Grush,et al.  The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  Cornelius J. König,et al.  Integrating Theories of Motivation , 2006 .

[19]  M. Frese,et al.  Action as the core of work psychology: A German approach. , 1994 .

[20]  Robert G. Lord,et al.  Effects of valence, expectancies, and goal-performance discrepancies in single and multiple goal environments. , 1990 .

[21]  W. V. Bingham,et al.  Expectancies , 1953 .

[22]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. , 1987 .

[23]  Rogelio Oliva,et al.  Model calibration as a testing strategy for system dynamics models , 2003, Eur. J. Oper. Res..

[24]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .

[25]  Christian Bierwirth,et al.  Principles of Systems , 2000 .

[26]  R. Dalal,et al.  Motivation for what? A multivariate dynamic perspective of the criterion. , 2008 .

[27]  L. Anderlini,et al.  Forecasting errors and bounded rationality: An example , 1998 .

[28]  Kathleen M. Carley,et al.  Simulation modeling in organizational and management research , 2007 .

[29]  Jeffrey B Vancouver,et al.  The effect of feedback sign on task performance depends on self-concept discrepancies. , 2004, The Journal of applied psychology.

[30]  Thomas W. Lee,et al.  Self-regulation and multiple deadline goals. , 2008 .

[31]  Bernhard Wilpert,et al.  Applied psychology : an international review , 1989 .

[32]  N. Taatgen,et al.  The problem state: a cognitive bottleneck in multitasking. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  E. A. Locke,et al.  A theory of goal setting & task performance , 1990 .

[34]  V. Vroom Work and motivation , 1964 .

[35]  C. Carver,et al.  On the Self-Regulation of Behavior , 1998 .

[36]  W. V. Eerde,et al.  Vroom's expectancy models and work-related criteria: A meta-analysis , 1996 .

[37]  Samuel B. Bacharach,et al.  Organizational Theories: Some Criteria for Evaluation , 1989 .

[38]  E. A. Locke,et al.  Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation. A 35-year odyssey. , 2002, The American psychologist.

[39]  A. Schmidt,et al.  What to do? The effects of discrepancies, incentives, and time on dynamic goal prioritization. , 2007, The Journal of applied psychology.

[40]  Lee Roy Beach,et al.  The Psychology of Decision Making: People in Organizations , 1997 .

[41]  R. Sternberg Advances in the psychology of human intelligence , 1982 .

[42]  A. Bandura Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. , 1977, Psychology Review.

[43]  J. Singer,et al.  Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis , 2003 .

[44]  R. Luce,et al.  Four tensions concerning mathematical modeling in psychology. , 1995, Annual review of psychology.

[45]  C. Carver,et al.  Attention and Self-Regulation: A Control-Theory Approach to Human Behavior , 1981 .

[46]  G. Clore,et al.  Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles , 1996 .

[47]  H. Klein,et al.  Goal choice and decision processes. , 2008 .

[48]  Arie W Kruglanski,et al.  Forgetting all else: on the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  J. B. Vancouver,et al.  If We Produce Discrepancies, Then How? Testing a Computational Process Model of Positive Goal Revision , 2010 .

[50]  R. Beck,et al.  Motivation: Theories and Principles , 1978 .

[51]  R. Buehler,et al.  Exploring the "planning fallacy": Why people underestimate their task completion times. , 1994 .

[52]  M. Forster,et al.  Key Concepts in Model Selection: Performance and Generalizability. , 2000, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[53]  A. K. Basu A Theory of Decision-Making , 1973, The Journal of Sociology & Social Welfare.

[54]  J. R. Landis,et al.  The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. , 1977, Biometrics.

[55]  Rik Pieters,et al.  Dynamics of multiple-goal pursuit. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[56]  A. Bandura Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy.

[57]  John M. Flach,et al.  Control Theory for Humans: Quantitative Approaches To Modeling Performance , 2002 .

[58]  K. Popper,et al.  Conjectures and refutations;: The growth of scientific knowledge , 1972 .

[59]  T. Kuhn,et al.  The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. , 1964 .

[60]  William. T. Powers,et al.  Living Control Systems II: Selected papers of William T , 1989 .

[61]  I. J. Myung,et al.  Tutorial on maximum likelihood estimation , 2003 .

[62]  P. Pintrich,et al.  Handbook of self-regulation , 2000 .

[63]  H. Klein,et al.  An Integrated Control Theory Model of Work Motivation , 1989 .

[64]  W. T. Powers Quantitative Analysis of Purposive Systems: Some Spadework at the Foundations of Scientific Psychology , 1978 .

[65]  J. Kitts,et al.  Computational Modeling , 2020, SAGE Research Methods Foundations.

[66]  Anthony S. Bryk,et al.  Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods , 1992 .

[67]  Tara A. Rench,et al.  Predictors of multitasking performance in a synthetic work paradigm , 2010 .

[68]  Charles A. Scherbaum,et al.  Do we self-regulate actions or perceptions? A test of two computational models , 2008, Comput. Math. Organ. Theory.

[69]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .

[70]  Daniel R. Ilgen,et al.  Introduction to computational modeling in organizations: The good that modeling does. , 2000 .

[71]  Jeffrey B. Vancouver,et al.  Using Dynamic Computational Models to Reconnect Theory and Research: Socialization by the Proactive Newcomer as Example , 2010 .

[72]  P. Hanges,et al.  A control system model of organizational motivation: Theoretical development and applied implications , 1987 .

[73]  William T. Powers,et al.  Living Control Systems: Selected Papers of William T. Powers , 1989 .

[74]  Robert D. Pritchard,et al.  Work motivation : past, present, and future , 2008 .

[75]  Carol T. Kulik,et al.  Old Friends, New Faces: Motivation Research in the 1990s , 1999 .

[76]  K. Lewin,et al.  Field Theory in Social Science: Selected Theoretical Papers , 1951 .

[77]  Jeffrey B. Vancouver,et al.  Integrating self-regulation theories of work motivation into a dynamic process theory , 2008 .

[78]  E. Higgins Making a good decision: value from fit. , 2000, The American psychologist.

[79]  M. A. Campion,et al.  A control systems conceptualization of the goal-setting and changing process , 1982 .

[80]  J. Vancouver Measuring Individual Differences in Content via Changing Person–Context Interaction , 2009, Industrial and Organizational Psychology.

[81]  Thomas Li-Ping Tang,et al.  Income, Money Ethic, Pay Satisfaction, Commitment, and Unethical Behavior: Is the Love of Money the Root of Evil for Hong Kong Employees? , 2003 .

[82]  Paul E. Levy,et al.  Moving from Cognition to Action: A Control Theory Perspective , 1994 .

[83]  Joseph P. Forgas,et al.  Affective Influences on Judgments and Behavior in Organizations: An Information Processing Perspective , 2001 .

[84]  J. Forgas,et al.  On Managing Moods: Evidence for the Role of Homeostatic Cognitive Strategies in Affect Regulation , 2002 .

[85]  Jeffrey B Vancouver,et al.  Self-efficacy and resource allocation: support for a nonmonotonic, discontinuous model. , 2008, The Journal of applied psychology.

[86]  T. Mitchell,et al.  Building Better Theory: Time and The Specification of When Things Happen , 2001 .

[87]  C. Pinder,et al.  Work Motivation in Organizational Behavior , 1997 .

[88]  J. Diefendorff,et al.  Goal-Striving and Self-Regulation Processes , 2008 .

[89]  J. Monterosso,et al.  Beyond discounting: possible experimental models of impulse control , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[90]  M. Ross,et al.  The motivational impact of temporal focus: thinking about the future and the past. , 1996, Annual review of psychology.

[91]  Jeffrey B. Vancouver,et al.  Self-Regulation in Organizational Settings: A Tale of Two Paradigms , 2000 .

[92]  J. T. Austin,et al.  Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content. , 1996 .

[93]  F. W. Irwin Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men , 1932, The Psychological Clinic.

[94]  Richard P. DeShon,et al.  A motivated action theory account of goal orientation. , 2005, The Journal of applied psychology.

[95]  M. D. Dunnette Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology , 2005 .

[96]  Charles A. Scherbaum,et al.  Testing a Computational Model of the Goal-Level Effect: An Example of a Neglected Methodology , 2005 .