Odor hedonics: connection with emotional response estimated by autonomic parameters.

The aim of this paper is to analyse the relationship between self-report hedonic evaluations and the physiological expression of emotion in response to odorants. We try to solve the following questions: (1) Is it possible to find any experimental evidence that the sense of smell is linked with emotion? (2) What kind of odorants can be distinguished by autonomic analysis? (3) Is there a link between hedonics and autonomic information? The effects of odorants on the emotional process were estimated, in terms of autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity. Fifteen subjects inhaled five odorants as olfactory stimuli: lavender (LAV), ethyl acetoacetate (EAA), camphor (CAM), acetic acid (AA) and butyric acid (BA). After inhaling the odorant, subjects were requested to fill out an 11-point hedonic scale to rate its pleasantness versus unpleasantness. ANS parameters were as follows: two electrodermal responses, skin potential (SP) and resistance (SR); two thermovascular parameters, skin blood flow (SBF) and skin temperature (ST); and two cardiorespiratory parameters; instantaneous respiratory frequency (IRF) and instantaneous heart rate (IHR). Simultaneous recording of six parameters showed that specific autonomic patterns were associated with each odorant. An analysis of variance made it possible to differentiate among the five odorants. Two-by-two odorant comparisons for autonomic responses using Tukey's HSD multiple comparison test only permitted differentiation between pleasant odorants (LAV and EAA) and unpleasant (AA and BA) ones, but camphor was differentiated from both pleasant and unpleasant odorants. Each odorant elicited responses in the different parameters, yet subjects responded through their preferential channels; an average of two channels was used by each subject. These results when compared with those obtained with other senses (visual and auditory), did not evidence the postulated preferential link between olfaction and emotion. A strong link between hedonics and ANS response could be demonstrated when considering each subject and mainly through his/her preferential channel(s); conversely a weak correlation (SR duration excepted) was obtained between inter-subjects' hedonic evaluation. It seems that for a given population the autonomic response reflect the odor valence only through some parameters related to the main preferential channel(s) and thus the global autonomic pattern has to be considered.

[1]  The contingent negative variation in an odor labeling paradigm. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[2]  C. Collet,et al.  A multi-sensor system for the non-invasive measurement of the activity of the autonomic nervous system , 1995 .

[3]  R. Soussignan,et al.  Facial responsiveness to odours in normal and pervasively developmentally disordered children. , 1995, Chemical senses.

[4]  A. Dittmar,et al.  The ohmic perturbation duration, an original temporal index to quantify electrodermal responses , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[5]  P. Brauchli,et al.  Electrocortical and autonomic alteration by administration of a pleasant and an unpleasant odor. , 1995, Chemical senses.

[6]  A Priez,et al.  Bioelectric and microcirculation cutaneous sensors for the study of vigilance and emotional response during tasks and tests. , 1995, Biosensors & bioelectronics.

[7]  T. Katsuura,et al.  [Responses of electroencephalogram to different odors]. , 1994, The Annals of physiological anthropology = Seiri Jinruigaku Kenkyukai kaishi.

[8]  C. Collet,et al.  Autonomic responses correlate to motor anticipation , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.

[9]  C. Braun,et al.  Emotional qualities of odors and their influence on the startle reflex in humans. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[10]  S. Vrana,et al.  The psychophysiology of disgust: differentiating negative emotional contexts with facial EMG. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[11]  A. Dittmar,et al.  A thermal conductivity sensor for the measurement of skin blood flow , 1992 .

[12]  H. Ehrlichman,et al.  Olfaction and Emotion , 1992 .

[13]  E. Koss,et al.  Perfumery: The Psychology and Biology of Fragrance , 1991 .

[14]  A. Dittmar,et al.  Analysis Of Skin Potential Response Using A Novel Feature Code For The Study Of The Emotional Response , 1991, Proceedings of the Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society Volume 13: 1991.

[15]  W. Hubert,et al.  Autonomic, neuroendocrine, and subjective responses to emotion-inducing film stimuli. , 1991, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[16]  H. Lawless,et al.  Effects of Odors on Mood and Behavior: Aromatherapy and Related Effects , 1991 .

[17]  John Sabini,et al.  Emotional State, Physical Well‐Being, and Performance in the Presence of Feigned Ambient Odor1 , 1990 .

[18]  Robert A. Baron,et al.  Environmentally Induced Positive Affect: Its Impact on Self‐Efficacy, Task Performance, Negotiation, and Conflict1 , 1990 .

[19]  M. Devos Standardized human olfactory thresholds , 1990 .

[20]  H. Ludvigson,et al.  Effects of ambient odors of lavender and cloves on cognition, memory, affect and mood , 1989 .

[21]  J. Richardson,et al.  Cognition and olfaction: a review. , 1989, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  G Kobal,et al.  Cerebral chemosensory evoked potentials elicited by chemical stimulation of the human olfactory and respiratory nasal mucosa. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[23]  M. Schleidt,et al.  Pleasure and disgust: memories and associations of pleasant and unpleasant odours in Germany and Japan , 1988 .

[24]  H. Ehrlichman,et al.  Affect and memory: effects of pleasant and unpleasant odors on retrieval of happy and unhappy memories. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  S. Toller,et al.  Emotion and the brain , 1988 .

[26]  R. Tisserand Essential oils as psychotherapeutic agents , 1988 .

[27]  S. Torii,et al.  Contingent negative variation (CNV) and the psychological effects of odour , 1988 .

[28]  Mortimer Mishkin,et al.  Chapter 12 – THE AMYGDALA: SENSORY GATEWAY TO THE EMOTIONS , 1986 .

[29]  R. Zajonc Emotions and facial expression. , 1985, Science.

[30]  C. Izard Emotions and facial expression. , 1985, Science.

[31]  J. Rotton,et al.  Affective and Cognitive Consequences of Malodorous Pollution , 1983 .

[32]  G. Dodd,et al.  Skin conductance and subjective assessments associated with the odour of 5-α-androstan-3-one , 1983, Biological Psychology.

[33]  J. E. Steiner,et al.  Human facial expressions in response to taste and smell stimulation. , 1979, Advances in child development and behavior.

[34]  J. Rotton,et al.  Air Pollution and interpersonal Attraction1 , 1978 .

[35]  P. Jurs,et al.  Intranasal trigeminal stimulation from odorous volatiles: Psychometric responses from anosmic and normal humans , 1978, Physiology & Behavior.

[36]  G. Mulder Methods and limits of psychophysiology. , 1973, Psychiatria, neurologia, neurochirurgia.

[37]  Ian Darian-Smith,et al.  The Trigeminal System , 1973 .

[38]  J. Wilder,et al.  BASIMETRIC APPROACH (LAW OF INITIAL VALUE) TO BIOLOGICAL RHYTHMS , 1962, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[39]  Borsanyl Sj,et al.  Psychogalvanic skin response as a method of objective olfactometry. , 1962 .

[40]  F. Baker,et al.  Psychogalvanic skin response as a method of objective olfactometry. , 1962, Bulletin of the School of Medicine.

[41]  C. Blanchard,et al.  Psychogalvanic skin response olfactometry. , 1962, The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology.

[42]  J. Lacey,et al.  Autonomic Response Specificity: An Experimental Study , 1953, Psychosomatic medicine.

[43]  H. Scholsberg A scale for the judgment of facial expressions. , 1941 .