Reading Ability and Utilization of Orthographic Structure in Reading

A perceptual-recognition task was used to assess whether utilization of orthographic structure in letter recognition varies with reading ability. Anagrams of words were made to create strings that orthogonally combined frequency and regularity measures of orthographic structure. These strings and the original words were used as test stimuli in a letter-recognition task. Good and poor college readers showed equally large effects of orthographic structure on task accuracy, whereas poor sixth-grade readers did not utilize orthographic structure to the same degree as very good sixth-grade readers. To facilitate the teaching of orthographic structure, some of the important constraints in written English and various games for teaching these constraints are presented. One of the primary concerns of educators is the wide range of reading ability across both child and adult populations. Good and poor readers have been shown to differ in performance on a variety of general tasks, but until recently little attempt has been made to explore how children of different reading abilities might differ in basic wordrecognition processes. If poor readers reveal fundamental deficits in certain recognition processes, then instruction for the poor readers could be directed at these processes.

[1]  D. Massaro,et al.  Letter and word perception : orthographic structure and visual processing in reading , 1980 .

[2]  D W Massaro,et al.  Letter information and orthographic context in word perception. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Glen A. Taylor,et al.  Reading Ability and the Utilization of Orthographic Structure in Reading. Technical Report No. 515. , 1979 .

[4]  D W Massaro,et al.  Orthographic regularity, positional frequency, and visual processing of letter strings. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[5]  Dominic W. Massaro,et al.  Reading and Listening (Tutorial Paper) , 1979 .

[6]  P. A. Weaver,et al.  Theory and practice of early reading , 1979 .

[7]  Dominic W. Massaro,et al.  Considerations in software character generation , 1978 .

[8]  Glen A. Taylor,et al.  A visual display system for reading and visual perception research , 1978 .

[9]  Dominic W. Massaro,et al.  A Stage Model of Reading and Listening. , 1978 .

[10]  M. Mason,et al.  Visual processing of nonlinguistic strings: redundancy effects and reading ability. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  W. Estes,et al.  Serial position functions for letter identification at brief and extended exposure durations , 1976 .

[12]  B G Breitmeyer,et al.  Implications of sustained and transient channels for theories of visual pattern masking, saccadic suppression, and information processing. , 1976, Psychological review.

[13]  M. Mason Reading Ability and Letter Search Time: Effects of Orthographic Structure Defined by Single-Letter Positional Frequency. , 1975 .

[14]  Dominic W. Massaro,et al.  Visual Features, Preperceptual Storage, and Processing Time in Reading , 1975 .

[15]  D. Massaro Perception of letters, words, and nonwords. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[16]  D. Massaro,et al.  Visual information and redundancy in reading. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  John B. Carroll,et al.  The American Heritage Word Frequency Book , 1971 .

[18]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[19]  M. S. Mayzner,et al.  Tables of single-letter and digram frequency counts for various word-length and letter-position combinations. , 1965 .

[20]  J. Bruner,et al.  Familiarity of Letter Sequences and Tachistoscopic Identification , 1954 .

[21]  Claude E. Shannon,et al.  Prediction and Entropy of Printed English , 1951 .

[22]  S. T. Orton WORD-BLINDNESS IN SCHOOL CHILDREN , 1925 .