Language decline across the life span: findings from the Nun Study.

The present study examines language samples from the Nun Study. Measures of grammatical complexity and idea density were obtained from autobiographies written over a 60-year span. Participants who had met criteria for dementia were contrasted with those who did not. Grammatical complexity initially averaged 4.78 (on a 0-to-7-point scale) for participants who did not meet criteria for dementia and declined .04 units per year; grammatical complexity for participants who met criteria for dementia initially averaged 3.86 and declined .03 units per year. Idea density averaged 5.35 propositions per 10 words initially for participants who did not meet criteria for dementia and declined an average of .03 units per year, whereas idea density averaged 4.34 propositions per 10 words initially for participants who met criteria for dementia and declined .02 units per year. Adult experiences, in general, did not moderate these declines.

[1]  K. Schaie,et al.  Age difference patterns of psychometric intelligence in adulthood: generalizability within and across ability domains. , 1993, Psychology and aging.

[2]  Randall W Engle,et al.  Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  W. Markesbery,et al.  Brain infarction and the clinical expression of Alzheimer disease. The Nun Study. , 1997, JAMA.

[4]  J. Ware,et al.  Random-effects models for longitudinal data. , 1982, Biometrics.

[5]  Hintat Cheung,et al.  Competing complexity metrics and adults' production of complex sentences , 1992, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[6]  H. Buschke,et al.  Cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships among age, cognition, and processing speed. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[7]  N. R. Schultz,et al.  Intellectual Functioning in Adults , 1974 .

[8]  D. Snowdon,et al.  Aging and Alzheimer's disease: lessons from the Nun Study. , 1997, The Gerontologist.

[9]  James A. Mortimer,et al.  Linguistic Ability in Early Life and Longevity: Findings from the Nun Study , 1999 .

[10]  M. Albert,et al.  Changes in naming ability with age. , 1988, Psychology and aging.

[11]  D R Wekstein,et al.  Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer's disease in late life. Findings from the Nun Study. , 1996, JAMA.

[12]  S. Kemper,et al.  The structure of verbal abilities in young and older adults. , 2001, Psychology and aging.

[13]  K. Warner Schaie,et al.  Longitudinal studies of adult psychological development , 1983 .

[14]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[15]  T. Arbuckle,et al.  Social behavior and off-target verbosity in elderly people. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[16]  Walter Kintsch,et al.  Reading rate and retention as a function of the number of propositions in the base structure of sentences , 1973 .

[17]  T. Arbuckle,et al.  Verbosity in Older Adults , 1994 .

[18]  S. Kemper,et al.  Longitudinal change in language production: effects of aging and dementia on grammatical complexity and propositional content. , 2001, Psychology and aging.

[19]  D. Hultsch,et al.  Memory Change in the Aged , 1998 .

[20]  D Arenberg Longitudinal changes in cognitive performance. , 1990, Advances in neurology.

[21]  Susan Kemper,et al.  Oral language and alzheimer's disease: A reduction in syntactic complexity , 1994 .

[22]  J. Morris,et al.  The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assesment of Alzheimer's disease , 1989, Neurology.

[23]  John M. Gottman,et al.  The Analysis of Change , 1995 .

[24]  K. Schaie Midlife Influences Upon Intellectual Functioning in Old Age , 1984 .

[25]  Judith D. Singer,et al.  Using SAS PROC MIXED to Fit Multilevel Models, Hierarchical Models, and Individual Growth Models , 1998 .

[26]  R. Dixon,et al.  Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[27]  H. Massie,et al.  Linguistic ability in early life and Alzheimer disease in late life. , 1996, JAMA.

[28]  Claudio Franceschi,et al.  The Paradoxes of Longevity , 1999, Research and Perspectives in Longevity.

[29]  Models of intelligence in late life: comment on Hultsch et al. (1999). , 1999 .

[30]  N. Cowan Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework , 1995 .

[31]  R. Brookmeyer,et al.  Predicting Rate of Cognitive Decline in Probable Alzheimer's Disease , 1996, Brain and Cognition.

[32]  R. Dixon,et al.  On the problem of detecting effects of lifestyle on cognitive change in adulthood: Reply to Pushkar et al. (1999). , 1999 .

[33]  N. Butters,et al.  The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part V. A normative study of the neuropsychological battery , 1994, Neurology.

[34]  F R Ferraro,et al.  On the preservation of syntax in Alzheimer's disease. Evidence from written sentences. , 1993, Archives of neurology.

[35]  Interpersonal Communication in Older Adulthood: Interdisciplinary Theory and Research , 1994 .

[36]  Alex E. Schwartzman,et al.  Structural equation model of intellectual change and continuity and predictors of intelligence in older men. , 1995 .

[37]  Sheldon Rosenberg,et al.  Indicators of linguistic competence in the peer group conversational behavior of mildly retarded adults , 1987, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[38]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The Boston naming test , 2001 .

[39]  N. Cowan Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  S. Kemper,et al.  Life-span changes to adults' language: Effects of memory and genre , 1989, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[41]  E. Zelinski,et al.  Sixteen-year longitudinal and time lag changes in memory and cognition in older adults. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[42]  Tom A. B. Snijders,et al.  Analysis of longitudinal data using the hierarchical linear model , 1996 .

[43]  R. Keefover,et al.  Aging and cognition. , 1998, Neurologic clinics.

[44]  T. Arbuckle,et al.  Measurement and correlates of verbosity in elderly people. , 1988, Journal of gerontology.

[45]  T. Arbuckle,et al.  Off-Target Verbosity and Talkativeness in Elderly People , 1993, Canadian Journal on Aging / La Revue canadienne du vieillissement.

[46]  D. Kempler,et al.  Syntactic preservation in Alzheimer's disease. , 1987, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[47]  H. Goldstein Multilevel Statistical Models , 2006 .

[48]  Jack Botwinick,et al.  Intellectual ability among the elderly: Simultaneous cross-sectional and longitudinal comparisons. , 1980 .

[49]  D Commenges,et al.  A 5-year longitudinal study of the Mini-Mental State Examination in normal aging. , 1997, American journal of epidemiology.

[50]  Dennis Wallace,et al.  Analysis of repeated measures designs with linear mixed models. , 2001 .

[51]  T. Arbuckle,et al.  A longitudinal study of Off-Target Verbosity. , 1995, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[52]  J. Hughes,et al.  Cognitive deterioration in Alzheimer's disease: behavioral and health factors. , 1990, Journal of gerontology.