Longitudinal Study of Performance on the Ruff Figural Fluency Test in Persons Aged 35 Years or Older

The Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT) is a cognitive test to measure executive function. Longitudinal studies have shown that repeated testing improves performance on the RFFT. Such a practice effect may hinder the interpretation of test results in a clinical setting. Therefore, we investigated the longitudinal performance on the RFFT in persons aged 35–82 years. Performance on the RFFT was measured three times over an average follow-up period of six years in 2,515 participants of the Prevention of REnal and Vascular ENd-stage Disease (PREVEND) study in Groningen, the Netherlands: 53% men; mean age (SD), 53 (10) years. The effect of consecutive measurements on performance on the RFFT was investigated with linear multilevel regression models that also included age, gender, educational level and the interaction term consecutive measurement number x age as independent variables. It was found that the mean (SD) number of unique designs on the RFFT increased from 73 (26) at the first measurement to 79 (27) at the second measurement and to 83 (26) at the third measurement (p<0.001). However, the increase per consecutive measurement number was negatively associated with age and decreased with 0.23 per one-year increment of age (p<0.001). The increase per consecutive measurement number was not dependent on educational level. Similar results were found for the median (IQR) number of perseverative errors which showed a small but statistically significant increase with repeating testing: 7 (3–13) at the first measurement, 7 (4–14) at the second measurement and 8 (4–15) at the third measurement (p trend = 0.002). In conclusion, the performance on the RFFT improved by repeating the test over an average follow-up period of three to six years. This practice effect was the largest in young adults and not dependent on educational level.

[1]  J. Slaets,et al.  The Interaction of Age and Type 2 Diabetes on Executive Function and Memory in Persons Aged 35 Years or Older , 2013, PloS one.

[2]  P. Vik,et al.  Strategy Use on the Ruff Figural Fluency Test☆ , 2013, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[3]  K. Lange,et al.  The Five-Point Test: Reliability, Validity and Normative Data for Children and Adults , 2012, PloS one.

[4]  D. Tranel,et al.  Scoring Higher the Second Time Around: Meta-Analyses of Practice Effects in Neuropsychological Assessment , 2012, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[5]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Effects of Age on Time-Dependent Cognitive Change , 2011, Psychological science.

[6]  J. Slaets,et al.  Reference Data for the Ruff Figural Fluency Test Stratified by Age and Educational Level , 2011, PloS one.

[7]  H. Ehrenreich,et al.  Practice effects in healthy adults: A longitudinal study on frequent repetitive cognitive testing , 2010, BMC Neuroscience.

[8]  A. Hillis,et al.  Predictors and assessment of cognitive dysfunction resulting from ischaemic stroke , 2010, The Lancet Neurology.

[9]  Hans L Hillege,et al.  Microalbuminuria and risk of venous thromboembolism. , 2009, JAMA.

[10]  H. Mehdorn,et al.  Normative data and psychometric properties for qualitative and quantitative scoring criteria of the Five-point Test , 2009, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[11]  B. Lebowitz,et al.  Reliability of the Five-Point Test , 2009, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[12]  D. de Zeeuw,et al.  Albuminuria assessed from first-morning-void urine samples versus 24-hour urine collections as a predictor of cardiovascular morbidity and mortality. , 2008, American journal of epidemiology.

[13]  Jelle Jolles,et al.  Detecting the significance of changes in performance on the Stroop Color-Word Test, Rey's Verbal Learning Test, and the Letter Digit Substitution Test: The regression-based change approach , 2007, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[14]  John P. Hausknecht,et al.  Retesting in selection: a meta-analysis of coaching and practice effects for tests of cognitive ability. , 2007, The Journal of applied psychology.

[15]  N. Raz,et al.  Differential Aging of the Brain: Patterns, Cognitive Correlates and Modifiers , 2022 .

[16]  D. Schretlen,et al.  Interrater and Test–Retest Reliability of a Fixed Condition Design Fluency Test , 2006, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[17]  E. Siemers,et al.  Practice effects and the use of alternate forms in serial neuropsychological testing. , 2005, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[18]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[19]  Emilio Ferrer,et al.  Estimating retest effects in longitudinal assessments of cognitive functioning in adults between 18 and 60 years of age. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[20]  T. Ross,et al.  The reliability of production strategy scores for the Ruff Figural Fluency Test. , 2003, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[21]  J. Bryan,et al.  Measurement of Executive Function: Considerations for Detecting Adult Age Differences , 2000, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[22]  R. Bornstein,et al.  Practice effects on commonly used measures of executive function across twelve months. , 1999, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[23]  G. Demakis Serial malingering on verbal and nonverbal fluency and memory measures: an analog investigation. , 1999, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[24]  P. Satz,et al.  Effect of repeated administration of a neuropsychological battery in the elderly. , 1991, Journal of clinical psychology.

[25]  O. Selnes A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests , 1991, Neurology.

[26]  Marianne Regard,et al.  Children's Production on Verbal and Non-Verbal Fluency Tasks , 1982, Perceptual and motor skills.

[27]  Brenda Milner,et al.  Design fluency: The invention of nonsense drawings after focal cortical lesions , 1977, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  W. Noyes United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization , 1946, International Organization.

[29]  D. Selkoe Alzheimer's disease. , 2011, Cold Spring Harbor perspectives in biology.

[30]  W. Sturm,et al.  Neuropsychological assessment , 2007, Journal of Neurology.

[31]  J. Yesavage,et al.  Fluency Performance Patterns in Alzheimer's Disease and Parkinson's Disease , 1998 .

[32]  Harry A. Whitaker,et al.  Early effects of normal aging on perseverative and non‐perseverative prefrontal measures , 1992 .

[33]  Ronald M. Ruff,et al.  The ruff figural fluency test: A normative study with adults , 1987 .

[34]  R. Ruff,et al.  Impaired verbal and figural fluency after head injury. , 1986, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.