Human control of an inverted pendulum: Is continuous control necessary? Is intermittent control effective? Is intermittent control physiological?

Homeostasis, the physiological control of variables such as body position, is founded on negative feedback mechanisms. The default understanding, consistent with a wealth of knowledge related to peripheral reflexes, is that feedback mechanisms controlling body position act continuously. For more than fifty years, it has been assumed that sustained control of position is best interpreted using continuous paradigms from engineering control theory such as those which regulate speed in a vehicle ‘cruise control’ system. Using a joystick to control an unstable load that falls over like a person fainting, we show that control using intermittent gentle taps is natural, more effective and robust to unexpected changes than continuous hand contact, works best with two taps per second, and can explain the upper frequency limit of control by both methods. Serial ballistic control, limited to an optimum rate, provides a new physiological paradigm for interpreting sustained control of posture and movement.

[1]  John G. Milton,et al.  Balancing with Vibration: A Prelude for “Drift and Act” Balance Control , 2009, PloS one.

[2]  Yoshiyuki Asai,et al.  A Model of Postural Control in Quiet Standing: Robust Compensation of Delay-Induced Instability Using Intermittent Activation of Feedback Control , 2009, PloS one.

[3]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Computational principles of sensorimotor control that minimize uncertainty and variability , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  F C T van der Helm,et al.  Observations from unperturbed closed loop systems cannot indicate causality. , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  M. D. Neilson,et al.  An overview of adaptive model theory: solving the problems of redundancy, resources, and nonlinear interactions in human movement control , 2005, Journal of neural engineering.

[6]  Tim Kiemel,et al.  Slow dynamics of postural sway are in the feedback loop. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  P. Rack,et al.  Response of the normal human ankle joint to imposed sinusoidal movements. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  J. Wessberg,et al.  Organization of motor output in slow finger movements in man. , 1993, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  Rik Pintelon,et al.  System Identification: A Frequency Domain Approach , 2012 .

[10]  P. D. Neilson,et al.  Stochastic prediction in pursuit tracking: An experimental test of adaptive model theory , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[11]  Peter J Gawthrop,et al.  Visual control of stable and unstable loads: what is the feedback delay and extent of linear time‐invariant control? , 2009, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Frequency-domain analysis of intermittent control , 2009 .

[13]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Changing your mind: a computational mechanism of vacillation , 2009, Nature.

[14]  Martin Lakie,et al.  Manually controlled human balancing using visual, vestibular and proprioceptive senses involves a common, low frequency neural process , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[15]  P. Rack,et al.  Reflex responses at the human ankle: the importance of tendon compliance. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[16]  Valentina Squeri,et al.  Force-Field Compensation in a Manual Tracking Task , 2010, PloS one.

[17]  K. J. Craik Theory of the human operator in control systems; man as an element in a control system. , 1948, British Journal of Psychology General Section.

[18]  P. D. Neilson,et al.  Internal models and intermittency: A theoretical account of human tracking behavior , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[19]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Sensorimotor attenuation by central motor command signals in the absence of movement , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  R. Miall,et al.  Intermittency in human manual tracking tasks. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  K. J. Craik THEORY OF THE HUMAN OPERATOR IN CONTROL SYSTEMS , 1948 .

[22]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Evidence for an error deadzone in compensatory tracking. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[23]  M Lakie,et al.  Thixotropic changes in human muscle stiffness and the effects of fatigue. , 1988, Quarterly journal of experimental physiology.

[24]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[25]  T. Kiemel,et al.  Identification of the plant for upright stance in humans: multiple movement patterns from a single neural strategy. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Ian David Loram,et al.  The frequency of human, manual adjustments in balancing an inverted pendulum is constrained by intrinsic physiological factors , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[27]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Human postural sway results from frequent, ballistic bias impulses by soleus and gastrocnemius , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[28]  George A. Bekey,et al.  Identification of Sampling Intervals in Sampled-Data Models of Human Operators , 1968 .

[29]  R. Peterka,et al.  A new interpretation of spontaneous sway measures based on a simple model of human postural control. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  K. J. W. Craik Theory of the human operator in control systems; the operator as an engineering system. , 1947 .

[31]  E. C. Poulton,et al.  Tracking skill and manual control , 1974 .

[32]  M. Hinder,et al.  The Case for an Internal Dynamics Model versus Equilibrium Point Control in Human Movement , 2003, The Journal of physiology.

[33]  Taishin Nomura,et al.  Bounded stability of the quiet standing posture: an intermittent control model. , 2008, Human movement science.

[34]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Intermittent model predictive control , 2007 .

[35]  L. Stark,et al.  Sampling or intermittency in hand control system dynamics. , 1968, Biophysical journal.

[36]  M. Vince The intermittency of control movements and the psychological refractory period. , 1948, The British journal of psychology. General section.

[37]  A M Amjad,et al.  A framework for the analysis of mixed time series/point process data--theory and application to the study of physiological tremor, single motor unit discharges and electromyograms. , 1995, Progress in biophysics and molecular biology.

[38]  Jacques Droulez,et al.  Does the brain use sliding variables for the control of movements? , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[39]  Constantinos N Maganaris,et al.  Active, non‐spring‐like muscle movements in human postural sway: how might paradoxical changes in muscle length be produced? , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[40]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Intermittent control: a computational theory of human control , 2011, Biological Cybernetics.

[41]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[42]  H. van der Kooij,et al.  Observations from unperturbed closed loop systems cannot indicate causality , 2005 .

[43]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Human balancing of an inverted pendulum: position control by small, ballistic‐like, throw and catch movements , 2002, The Journal of physiology.

[44]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Open-loop intermittent feedback control: practical continuous-time GPC , 1999 .

[45]  K.J. Hunt,et al.  New results in feedback control of unsupported standing in paraplegia , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.

[46]  M. Lakie,et al.  A cross‐bridge mechanism can explain the thixotropic short‐range elastic component of relaxed frog skeletal muscle , 1998, The Journal of physiology.

[47]  Herman van der Kooij,et al.  Postural responses evoked by platform pertubations are dominated by continuous feedback. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Intermittent Predictive Control of an Inverted Pendulum , 2006 .

[49]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Mere Expectation to Move Causes Attenuation of Sensory Signals , 2008, PloS one.

[50]  Constantinos N Maganaris,et al.  The passive, human calf muscles in relation to standing: the short range stiffness lies in the contractile component , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[51]  Ian D Loram,et al.  Changes in joint angle, muscle‐tendon complex length, muscle contractile tissue displacement, and modulation of EMG activity during acute whole‐body vibration , 2009, Muscle & nerve.

[52]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning , 1998, Nature.

[53]  K. Newell,et al.  Intermittency in the control of continuous force production. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Attenuation of Self-Generated Tactile Sensations Is Predictive, not Postdictive , 2006, PLoS biology.

[55]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Event-driven intermittent control , 2009, Int. J. Control.

[56]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Human balancing of an inverted pendulum with a compliant linkage: neural control by anticipatory intermittent bias , 2003, The Journal of physiology.

[57]  L. Pinneo On noise in the nervous system. , 1966, Psychological review.

[58]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Human balancing of an inverted pendulum: is sway size controlled by ankle impedance? , 2001, The Journal of physiology.

[59]  Thomas Mergner,et al.  Sensory contributions to the control of stance: a posture control model. , 2002, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.