Taking the laboratory to the skies: ambulatory assessment of self-report, autonomic, and respiratory responses in flying phobia.

We evaluated the feasibility of recording multiple physiological anxiety measures during a flight and how well they could distinguish flight phobics from controls. Benefits of baseline adjustment and transformation for all variables and adjustment of heart rate by ventilation to give additional heart rate were calculated. Effect size, one measure of the power to discriminate groups, was between 1.1 and 1.7 for heart rate measures. Although respiratory rate and minute ventilation, indicators of hyperventilation, did not differ between groups, phobics paused more during inspiration than did controls. Phobics also showed more skin conductance fluctuations and less respiratory sinus arrhythmia. Self-reported anxiety was a more powerful discriminator than physiological measures, a result that may be partially explained by how phobics were selected. These results indicate that monitoring of multiple physiological systems outside the laboratory is practical and informative. Physiological measures of psychological importance can be quantified accurately in a noisy, changing, unsupervised ambulatory setting.

[1]  B. T. Engel Stimulus-Response and Individual-Response Specificity , 1960 .

[2]  P. Welch The use of fast Fourier transform for the estimation of power spectra: A method based on time averaging over short, modified periodograms , 1967 .

[3]  Peter J. Lang,et al.  Fear reduction and fear behavior: Problems in treating a construct. , 1968 .

[4]  H. Ursin,et al.  Additional heart rate--an indicator of psychological activation. , 1974, Aerospace medicine.

[5]  G. Prigatano,et al.  Autonomic nervous system changes associated with a spider phobic reaction. , 1974, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[6]  S. Rachman The conditioning theory of fearacquisition: A critical examination , 1977 .

[7]  F. Graham,et al.  Constraints on measuring heart rate and period sequentially through real and cardiac time. , 1978, Psychophysiology.

[8]  P. Venables,et al.  Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. , 1981 .

[9]  M. Sackner,et al.  Validation of respiratory inductive plethysmography using different calibration procedures. , 2015, The American review of respiratory disease.

[10]  M Lader,et al.  Ambulatory electrodermal monitoring: effects of ambient temperature, general activity, electrolyte media, and length of recording. , 1983, Psychophysiology.

[11]  D. Carroll,et al.  Heart rate and oxygen consumption during mental arithmetic, a video game, and graded exercise: further evidence of metabolically-exaggerated cardiac adjustments? , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[12]  R. Ley Blood, breath, and fears: A hyperventilation theory of panic attacks and agoraphobia. , 1985 .

[13]  R. Nesse,et al.  Endocrine and cardiovascular responses during phobic anxiety. , 1985, Psychosomatic medicine.

[14]  R. Freedman,et al.  Ambulatory monitoring of panic disorder. , 1985, Archives of general psychiatry.

[15]  L. Hedges,et al.  Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis , 1987 .

[16]  D. Carroll,et al.  Heart rate and oxygen consumption during active psychological challenge: the effects of level of difficulty. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[17]  Adolf Pfefferbaum,et al.  Autonomic characteristics of agoraphobia with panic attacks , 1986, Biological Psychiatry.

[18]  I. Marks Fears, phobias, and rituals , 1987 .

[19]  M. Drake Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed. rev.) , 1988 .

[20]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[21]  K. Carlberg,et al.  Use of graded exercise to evaluate physiological hyperreactivity to mental stress. , 1991, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[22]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Autonomic determinism: the modes of autonomic control, the doctrine of autonomic space, and the laws of autonomic constraint. , 1991, Psychological review.

[23]  P. Grossman,et al.  Prediction of tonic parasympathetic cardiac control using respiratory sinus arrhythmia: the need for respiratory control. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[24]  D. Shapiro,et al.  Continuous physiological changes and subjective reports in panic patients: A preliminary methodological report , 1992, Biological Psychiatry.

[25]  Richard Balon,et al.  Decreased heart rate variability in panic disorder patients: A study of power-spectral analysis of heart rate , 1993, Psychiatry Research.

[26]  N. Frijda,et al.  Emotions and respiratory patterns: review and critical analysis. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[27]  M. Stein,et al.  Irregular breathing during sleep in patients with panic disorder. , 1995, The American journal of psychiatry.

[28]  J. Thayer,et al.  Vagal tone in generalized anxiety disorder and the effects of aversive imagery and worrisome thinking , 1995 .

[29]  J. Gorman,et al.  Ambulatory monitoring of respiration in anxiety. , 1996, Anxiety.

[30]  F. H. Wilhelm,et al.  Ambulatory assessment of clinical anxiety , 1996 .

[31]  W. Roth,et al.  Acute and delayed effects of alprazolam on flight phobics during exposure. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[32]  W. Roth,et al.  Autonomic instability during relaxation in panic disorder , 1998, Psychiatry Research.

[33]  Frank H Wilhelm,et al.  Trusting computerized data reduction too much: a critique of Anderson's ambulatory respiratory monitor , 1998, Biological Psychology.