On use of a nominal internal model to detect a loss of balance in a maximal forward reach.

We hypothesize that the CNS detects a loss of balance by comparing outputs predicted by a nominal, forward internal model with actual sensory outputs. When the resulting control error signal reaches an anomalously large value, this control error anomaly (CEA) signals a loss of balance and precedes any observable compensatory response. To test this hypothesis, a multi-input, multi-output internal model of a standing forward reach task was developed that incorporated on-line model identification and a Gaussian failure detection algorithm. Eleven healthy young women were then asked to stand and reach forward to a target positioned from 95 to 125% of their maximum reach distance. Kinematic and kinetic data were recorded at 100 Hz unilaterally from the upper body, leg, and foot. Evidence of successful CEA detection was a compensatory step between 100 ms and 2 s later. The results show that use of a threshold, set at 3 SD from the mean, on error in the control of leg segment acceleration detected a CEA and correctly predicted a compensatory response in 92.6% of 108 trials. Leg acceleration control error was a better predictor than upper body or foot acceleration control error (P = 0.000). CEA detection performed more reliably than loss of balance detection algorithms based on kinematic thresholds (P = 0.000). The results support the hypothesis that a loss of balance may be identified via the use of a nominal forward internal model and probabilistic error monitoring.

[1]  G. Wu,et al.  Distinguishing fall activities from normal activities by velocity characteristics. , 2000, Journal of biomechanics.

[2]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  E Bizzi,et al.  Motor learning by field approximation. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  R. Ivry,et al.  Cerebellar involvement in anticipating the consequences of self-produced actions during bimanual movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Does the brain model Newton's laws? , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  James A Ashton-Miller,et al.  Effects of age and gender on toe flexor muscle strength. , 2002, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[7]  N. Alexander,et al.  The effect of age and movement speed on maximum forward reach from an elevated surface: a study in healthy women. , 2003, Clinical biomechanics.

[8]  B E Maki,et al.  Temporal properties of attention sharing consequent to disturbed balance. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[9]  D. Marr A theory of cerebellar cortex , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[10]  Ksenia Kozak,et al.  On the control of balance during the performance of a forward reach: Effects of age, biomechanical and psychological factors. , 1999 .

[11]  J. Flanagan,et al.  The Inertial Anisotropy of the Arm Is Accurately Predicted during Movement Planning , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Dyscoordination of pinch and lift forces during grasp in patients with cerebellar lesions , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[14]  R. Cham,et al.  Changes in gait when anticipating slippery floors. , 2002, Gait & Posture.

[15]  S. Studenski,et al.  Functional reach: a new clinical measure of balance. , 1990, Journal of gerontology.

[16]  Paavo V. Komi,et al.  Reaction time and electromyographic activity during a sprint start , 2004, European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology.

[17]  L. E. Powell,et al.  The Activities-specific Balance Confidence (ABC) Scale. , 1995, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[18]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  James A Ashton-Miller,et al.  Effect of age on detecting a loss of balance in a seated whole-body balancing task. , 2005, Clinical biomechanics.

[20]  Constantinos N Maganaris,et al.  Paradoxical muscle movement in human standing , 2004, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  E. T. Hsiao,et al.  Common protective movements govern unexpected falls from standing height. , 1997, Journal of biomechanics.

[22]  M. Tinetti,et al.  Falls efficacy as a measure of fear of falling. , 1990, Journal of gerontology.

[23]  H Okada,et al.  Brain activation during maintenance of standing postures in humans. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  L. Nashner A model describing vestibular detection of body sway motion. , 1971, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[25]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[26]  Gordon S. Smith,et al.  COST OF INJURY IN THE UNITED STATES: A REPORT TO CONGRESS , 1989 .

[27]  Jakob Hohwy,et al.  Can neuroscience explain consciousness , 2004 .

[28]  D. Winter,et al.  Strategies for recovery from a trip in early and late swing during human walking , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Frans C. T. van der Helm,et al.  An adaptive model of sensory integration in a dynamic environment applied to human stance control , 2001, Biological Cybernetics.

[30]  Y. Pai,et al.  Center of mass velocity-position predictions for balance control. , 1997, Journal of biomechanics.

[31]  B E Maki,et al.  Cognitive demands of executing postural reactions: does aging impede attention switching? , 2001, Neuroreport.

[32]  Y C Pai,et al.  Thresholds for step initiation induced by support-surface translation: a dynamic center-of-mass model provides much better prediction than a static model. , 2000, Journal of biomechanics.

[33]  P. Leva Adjustments to Zatsiorsky-Seluyanov's segment inertia parameters. , 1996 .

[34]  Herman van der Kooij,et al.  A multisensory integration model of human stance control , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[35]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[37]  James A Ashton-Miller,et al.  Is a "loss of balance" a control error signal anomaly? Evidence for three-sigma failure detection in young adults. , 2004, Gait & posture.

[38]  R. Shadmehr Generalization as a behavioral window to the neural mechanisms of learning internal models. , 2004, Human movement science.

[39]  Ovidiu Lungu,et al.  Probability detection mechanisms and motor learning , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Delusions of alien control in the normal brain , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  D. Wolpert,et al.  The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action , 1999, Neuroreport.

[42]  M. Alexander,et al.  Principles of Neural Science , 1981 .