A common mechanism for the perception of first-order and second-order apparent motion

A common mechanism for perceiving first-order, luminance-defined, and second-order, texture-contrast defined apparent motion between two element locations is indicated by: (1) transitivity--whether or not motion is perceived is inter-changeably affected by activationally equivalent luminance and contrast changes at each location, (2) local integration--whether or not motion is perceived depends on the net activation change resulting from simultaneous background-relative luminance and background-relative contrast changes at the same element location, and (3) inseparability--apparent motion is not perceived through independent first- or second-order mechanisms when luminance and contrast co-vary at the same location. These results, which are predicted by the response characteristics of directionally selective cells in areas V1, MT, and MST, are not instead attributable to changes in the location of the most salient element (third-order motion), attentive feature tracking, or artifactual first-order motion. Their inconsistency with Lu and Sperling's [Lu, Z., Sperling, G. (1995a). Attention-generated apparent motion. Nature 377, 237, Lu, Z., Sperling, G. (2001). Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 18, 2331] model, which specifies independent first- and second-order mechanisms, may be due to computational requirements particular to the motion of discrete objects with distinct boundaries defined by spatial differences in luminance, texture contrast, or both.

[1]  A. Johnston,et al.  A unified account of three apparent motion illusions , 1995, Vision Research.

[2]  S Anstis,et al.  Second-Order Texture Contrast Resolves Ambiguous Apparent Motion , 1995, Perception.

[3]  THOMAS V. PAPATHOMAS,et al.  Precise Assessment of the Mean Effective Luminance of Texture Patches—An Approach Based on Reverse-phi Motion , 1996, Vision Research.

[4]  D. Tolhurst,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for sustained and transient detectors in human vision , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  T. Albright,et al.  Neuronal responses to edges defined by luminance vs. temporal texture in macaque area V1 , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[6]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Interattribute apparent motion , 1989, Vision Research.

[7]  K. Turano Evidence for a Common Motion Mechanism of Luminance-Modulated and Contrast-Modulated Patterns: Selective Adaptation , 1991, Perception.

[8]  A J Ahumada,et al.  Model of human visual-motion sensing. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[9]  Keith Langley,et al.  Computational analysis of non-Fourier motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[10]  H. Barlow,et al.  The mechanism of directionally selective units in rabbit's retina. , 1965, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  Andrew B. Watson,et al.  Apparent motion occurs only between similar spatial frequencies , 1986, Vision Research.

[12]  W. Reichardt,et al.  Autocorrelation, a principle for the evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system , 1961 .

[13]  G. Sperling,et al.  The dimensionality of texture-defined motion: a single channel theory , 1993, Vision Research.

[14]  Lee A. Gilroy,et al.  Counter-Changing Luminance: A Non-Fourier, Nonattentional Basis for the Perception of Single-Element Apparent Motion , 2002 .

[15]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Motion defined exclusively by second-order characteristics does not evoke optokinetic nystagmus , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[16]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Evidence for separate motion-detecting mechanisms for first- and second-order motion in human vision , 1994, Vision Research.

[17]  ANDREW T SMITH,et al.  Separate Detection of Moving Luminance and Contrast Modulations: Fact or Artifact? , 1997, Vision Research.

[18]  G. Mather,et al.  Evidence for second-order motion detectors , 1993, Vision Research.

[19]  S. Anstis,et al.  Effects of Luminance and Contrast on Direction of Ambiguous Apparent Motion , 1985, Perception.

[20]  P Cavanagh,et al.  Attention-based motion perception. , 1992, Science.

[21]  S. Gunnar O. Johansson,et al.  Configurations in event perception : an experimental study , 1951 .

[22]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Visual detection of motion , 1994 .

[23]  A. Johnston,et al.  Speed discrimination thresholds for first- and second-order bars and edges , 1997, Vision Research.

[24]  Aijaz A. Baloch,et al.  Neural model of first-order and second-order motion perception and magnocellular dynamics. , 1999, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[25]  Colin W. G. Clifford,et al.  PII: S0042-6989(98)00082-0 , 1998 .

[26]  T. Albright,et al.  Motion coherency rules are form-cue invariant , 1992, Vision Research.

[27]  Allan Pantle,et al.  An energy model of interframe interval effects in single-step apparent motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[28]  S. Nishida Spatiotemporal properties of motion perception for random-check contrast modulations , 1993, Vision Research.

[29]  N. Scott-Samuel,et al.  No local cancellation between directionally opposed first-order and second-order motion signals , 2000, Vision Research.

[30]  Duje Tadin,et al.  Visual coherence of moving and stationary image changes , 2002, Vision Research.

[31]  A. Pantle,et al.  On the mechanism that encodes the movement of contrast variations: Velocity discrimination , 1989, Vision Research.

[32]  J J Knierim,et al.  Neural responses to visual texture patterns in middle temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Mary M. Conte,et al.  Coherence and transparency of moving plaids composed of Fourier and non-Fourier gratings , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  R A Andersen,et al.  The Analysis of Complex Motion Patterns by Form/Cue Invariant MSTd Neurons , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Mary M. Conte,et al.  Nonlinear Preprocessing in Short-range Motion , 1997, Vision Research.

[36]  G. Sperling,et al.  The functional architecture of human visual motion perception , 1995, Vision Research.

[37]  J. Movshon,et al.  Receptive field organization of complex cells in the cat's striate cortex. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.

[38]  A. Johnston,et al.  Perceived motion of contrast-modulated gratings: Predictions of the multi-channel gradient model and the role of full-wave rectification , 1995, Vision Research.

[39]  H. Hock,et al.  Detection of counter-changing contrast: second-order apparent motion without postrectification motion-energy analysis or salience mapping/feature tracking. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  G Sperling,et al.  Measuring the amplification of attention. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  T D Albright,et al.  Form-cue invariant motion processing in primate visual cortex. , 1992, Science.

[42]  B. B. Lee,et al.  Sensitivity of macaque retinal ganglion cells to chromatic and luminance flicker. , 1989, The Journal of physiology.

[43]  Z L Lu,et al.  Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[44]  H. Hock,et al.  Dynamic, state-dependent thresholds for the perception of single-element apparent motion: Bistability from local cooperativity , 1997, Perception & Psychophysics.

[45]  L. P. O'Keefe,et al.  Processing of first- and second-order motion signals by neurons in area MT of the macaque monkey , 1998, Visual Neuroscience.

[46]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[47]  G. Mather Motion detector models: psychophysical evidence , 1994 .

[48]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[49]  R Sekuler,et al.  Contrast response of human visual mechanismsensitive to orientation and direction of motion. , 1969, Vision research.

[50]  C L Baker,et al.  A processing stream in mammalian visual cortex neurons for non-Fourier responses. , 1993, Science.

[51]  George Sperling,et al.  Attention-generated apparent motion , 1995, Nature.

[52]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Spatiotemporal receptive field organization in the lateral geniculate nucleus of cats and kittens. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Limits of attentive tracking reveal temporal properties of attention , 2000, Vision Research.