How reaction time measures elucidate the matching bias and the way negations are processed

Matching bias refers to the non-normative performance that occurs when elements mentioned in a rule do not correspond with those in a test item (e.g., consider the double mismatch between the rule If there is a not a T on the card then there is not a 4 and a card showing H6). One aim of the present work is to capture matching bias via reaction times as participants carry out truth-table evaluation tasks. Experiment 1 requires participants to verify conditional rules, and Experiment 2 to falsify them as the paradigm (a) employs four types of conditional sentences that systematically rotate negatives in the antecedent and consequent; and (b) presents predominantly cases having true antecedents. These experiments reveal that mismatching is linked to higher rates of incorrect responses and slower evaluation times. A second aim is to investigate the way not is processed. We compare a narrow view of negations, which argues that negation only denies information (e.g., not-T only says there is no T), to a search for alternatives view, which says that negations function to prime appropriate alternatives (e.g., not-T primes a search for other letters). Findings from both experiments support a narrow reading view.

[1]  H. Yama Matching versus optimal data selection in the Wason selection task , 2001 .

[2]  S. E. Newstead,et al.  Language and reasoning: a study of temporal factors , 1977, Cognition.

[3]  H. H. Clark The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. , 1973 .

[4]  K. Manktelow,et al.  Facilitation of reasoning by realism: Effect or non‐effect? , 1979 .

[5]  Laure Zago,et al.  Shifting from the Perceptual Brain to the Logical Brain: The Neural Impact of Cognitive Inhibition Training , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Jonathan St. B. T. Evans,et al.  Matching bias in conditional reasoning : Do we understand it after 25 years ? , 1998 .

[7]  Jonathan Evans,et al.  The Influence of Linguistic Form on Reasoning: The Case of Matching Bias , 1999 .

[8]  M. Oaksford,et al.  Reasoning with conditionals containing negated constituents. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  M. Oaksford Contrast classes and matching bias as explanations of the effects of negation on conditional reasoning , 2002 .

[10]  J. S. Evans,et al.  Interpretation and Matching Bias in a Reasoning Task , 1972 .

[11]  P. Wason The contexts of plausible denial , 1965 .

[12]  John Clibbens,et al.  The Role of Implicit and Explicit Negation in Conditional Reasoning Bias , 1996 .

[13]  CONDITIONAL REASONING WITH NEGATIONS : MATCHING BIAS AND IMPLICIT VERSUS EXPLICIT AFFIRMATION OR DENIAL , 1999 .

[14]  J. S. Evans,et al.  Matching bias in the selection task. , 1973 .

[15]  Thomas C. Ormerod,et al.  Reasoning with three Types of Conditional: Biases and Mental Models , 1993 .

[16]  Jonathan St. B. T. Evans,et al.  Deciding before you think: Relevance and reasoning in the selection task , 1996 .

[17]  G. Chierchia,et al.  Linguistic-pragmatic factors in interpreting disjunctions , 2002 .

[18]  Ira A. Noveck,et al.  To What Extent Do Pragmatic Reasoning Schemas Affect Performance on Wason's Selection Task? , 1996 .

[19]  D. Sperber,et al.  Relevance theory explains the selection task , 1995, Cognition.