A study of the efficacy of a comprehensive memory enhancement program in healthy elderly persons

Well educated, high functioning older adults (ages 60-90) were given a comprehensive memory enhancement training program to determine the effectiveness of the program in increasing cognitive performance and positively influencing self-assessments of memory efficacy. The 68 subjects who participated in the memory enhancement training were compared to 74 subjects who were enrolled in a video control group. Between subject differences were analyzed prior to the interventions and at three time points following the interventions (immediately post, 3 months and 6 months). Only one cognitive measure, assessing verbal memory, revealed a significant difference between the groups, with participants in the memory enhancement group showing less decline from baseline than the video control group immediately following the intervention but with no difference between groups at 6 months post-intervention. Several self-report measures showed that the memory enhancement training subjects experienced improved memory functioning and decreased memory concerns relative to the video control group. The self-reported effects of the training were sustained over the 6-month follow-up period.

[1]  E. Kaplan,et al.  Alternate form of the california verbal learning test: Development and reliability , 1991 .

[2]  M. D. Murphy,et al.  Training older adult free recall rehearsal strategies. , 1981, Journal of gerontology.

[3]  J. Morris,et al.  The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assesment of Alzheimer's disease , 1989, Neurology.

[4]  D. Wechsler WAIS-R manual : Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised , 1981 .

[5]  C Hertzog,et al.  Relationships between metamemory, memory predictions, and memory task performance in adults. , 1990, Psychology and aging.

[6]  H. Goodglass,et al.  Assessing visual memory disorders. , 1989 .

[7]  J. Yesavage,et al.  Long‐term efficacy of cognitive training for age‐associated memory impairment: A six‐month follow‐up study , 1986 .

[8]  J. Bienias,et al.  A three-year follow-up of older adult participants in a memory-skills training program. , 1988, Psychology and aging.

[9]  M. Storandt,et al.  Memory-skills training, memory complaints, and depression in older adults. , 1985, Journal of gerontology.

[10]  Reinhold Kliegl,et al.  On the locus and process of magnification of age differences during mnemonic training. , 1990 .

[11]  M. Lachman,et al.  Improving memory and control beliefs through cognitive restructuring and self-generated strategies. , 1992, Journal of gerontology.

[12]  Reinhold Kliegl,et al.  Testing-the-Limits and the Study of Adult Age Differences in Cognitive Plasticity of a Mnemonic Skill , 1989 .

[13]  Luc Goossens,et al.  Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: a meta-analytic study. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[14]  Elizabeth M. Zelinski,et al.  The Memory Functioning Questionnaire for assessment of memory complaints in adulthood and old age. , 1990 .

[15]  L. Poon,et al.  Individual Variability in Memory Training with the Elderly. , 1982 .

[16]  W. Rosen,et al.  Verbal fluency in aging and dementia , 1980 .

[17]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The Boston naming test , 2001 .

[18]  M. Folstein,et al.  Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. , 1993, JAMA.

[19]  R. Markley,et al.  Remembering mnemonics: a three-year follow-up on the effects of mnemonics training in elderly adults. , 1987, Experimental aging research.

[20]  G. Bower,et al.  Interactive imagery and affective judgments improve face-name learning in the elderly. , 1983, Journal of gerontology.

[21]  M. Lachman,et al.  Effects of control beliefs and attributions on memory self-assessments and performance. , 1987, Psychology and aging.

[22]  P. Satz,et al.  An abbreviation of the WAIS for clinical use. , 1962, Journal of clinical psychology.

[23]  K. Schaie,et al.  Training the elderly on the ability factors of spatial orientation and inductive reasoning. , 1986, Psychology and aging.

[24]  A. S. Neely,et al.  Long-Term Maintenance of Gains From Memory Training in Older Adults: Two 3½Year Follow-up Studies , 1993 .

[25]  R. West,et al.  The link between memory performance, self-assessment, and affective status. , 1984, Experimental aging research.

[26]  G. Rebok,et al.  Memory self-efficacy and performance differences in young and old adults: The effect of mnemonic training. , 1989 .

[27]  Margie E. Lachman,et al.  Assessing memory control beliefs: The memory controllability inventory , 1995 .

[28]  L. Bäckman,et al.  Effects of multifactorial memory training in old age: generalizability across tasks and individuals. , 1995, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[29]  J A Yesavage,et al.  Imagery pretraining and memory training in the elderly. , 1983, Gerontology.

[30]  A. Bandura Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. , 1977, Psychological review.

[31]  J. Yesavage Relaxation and memory training in 39 elderly patients. , 1984, The American journal of psychiatry.

[32]  P. Baltes,et al.  Effects of cognitive training and testing on intellectual efficacy beliefs in elderly adults. , 1991, Journal of gerontology.

[33]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[34]  L. Bäckman,et al.  MAINTENANCE OF GAINS FOLLOWING MULTIFACTORIAL AND UNIFACTORIAL MEMORY TRAINING IN LATE ADULTHOOD , 1993 .