Kinetics of stone tool production among novice and expert tool makers.

OBJECTIVES As is the case among many complex motor tasks that require prolonged practice before achieving expertise, aspects of the biomechanics of knapping vary according to the relative experience/skill level of the practitioner. In archaeological experiments focused on the production of Plio-Pleistocene stone tools, these skill-mediated biomechanical differences have bearings on experimental design, the interpretation of results, and lithic assemblage analysis. A robust body of work exists on variation in kinematic patterns across skill levels but less is known about potential kinetic differences. The current study was undertaken to better understand kinetic patterns observed across skill levels during "Oldowan," freehand stone tool production. MATERIALS AND METHODS Manual pressure data were collected from 23 novice and 9 expert stone tool makers during the production of simple stone flakes using direct hard hammer percussion. RESULTS Results show that expert tool makers experienced significantly lower cumulative pressure magnitudes and pressure-time integral magnitudes compared with novices. In expert knappers, digits I and II experienced similarly high pressures (both peak pressure and pressure-time integrals) and low variability in pressure relative to digits III-V. Novices, in contrast, tended to hold hammerstones such that pressure patterns were similar across digits II-V, and they showed low variability on digit I only. DISCUSSION The similar and consistent emphasis of the thumb by both skill groups indicates the importance of this digit in stabilizing the hammerstone. The emphasis placed on digit II is exclusive to expert knappers, and so this digit may offer osteological signals diagnostic of habitual expert tool production.

[1]  H. Dibble,et al.  A New Synthesis of Middle Paleolithic Variability , 1990, American Antiquity.

[2]  Jay S. Reti Quantifying Oldowan Stone Tool Production at Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania , 2016, PloS one.

[3]  Stephen R. Merritt,et al.  Hand grip diversity and frequency during the use of Lower Palaeolithic stone cutting-tools. , 2018, Journal of human evolution.

[4]  Daniel E Lieberman,et al.  Hand biomechanics during simulated stone tool use. , 2011, Journal of human evolution.

[5]  Gilles Dietrich,et al.  The role of expertise in tool use: skill differences in functional action adaptations to task constraints. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Wild capuchin monkeys spontaneously adjust actions when using hammer stones of different mass to crack nuts of different resistance. , 2016, American journal of physical anthropology.

[7]  A. Minetti,et al.  Himalayan porter's specialization: metabolic power, economy, efficiency and skill , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  Matthew W. Tocheri,et al.  The evolutionary history of the hominin hand since the last common ancestor of Pan and Homo , 2008, Journal of anatomy.

[9]  Kathryn Weedman Arthur,et al.  Feminine Knowledge and Skill Reconsidered: Women and Flaked Stone Tools , 2010 .

[10]  Josep Maria Vergès,et al.  What novice knappers have to learn to become expert stone toolmakers , 2010 .

[11]  Stephen J. Lycett,et al.  Toolstone constraints on knapping skill: Levallois reduction with two different raw materials , 2011 .

[12]  Kohske Takahashi,et al.  Create Elegant Data Visualisations Using the Grammar of Graphics [R package ggplot2 version 3.3.2] , 2020 .

[13]  T. Chaminade,et al.  The evolutionary neuroscience of tool making , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Christopher J. Dunmore,et al.  The unexpected importance of the fifth digit during stone tool production , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[15]  D. Stout,et al.  Knowledge vs. know-how? Dissecting the foundations of stone knapping skill. , 2020, Journal of human evolution.

[16]  H. Dibble,et al.  The Effect of Hammer Mass and Velocity on Flake Mass , 1995 .

[17]  A. Mateos,et al.  Energy Cost of Stone Knapping , 2018, Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory.

[18]  Christopher J. Dunmore,et al.  Trabecular variation in the first metacarpal and manipulation in hominids. , 2019, American journal of physical anthropology.

[19]  L. Leakey Recent Discoveries at Olduvai Gorge , 1960, Nature.

[20]  C. Boesch,et al.  Energetic cost of nut-cracking behaviour in wild chimpanzees , 1993 .

[21]  Nicholas B. Stephens,et al.  Trabecular bone patterning across the human hand. , 2018, Journal of human evolution.

[22]  J. Shea,et al.  Going big versus going small: Lithic miniaturization in hominin lithic technology , 2019, Evolutionary anthropology.

[23]  Justin Pargeter,et al.  Understanding stone tool-making skill acquisition: Experimental methods and evolutionary implications. , 2019, Journal of human evolution.

[24]  M W Marzke,et al.  Origin of the human hand. , 1971, American journal of physical anthropology.

[25]  Ella Assaf,et al.  Knowledge transmission and apprentice flint-knappers in the Acheulo-Yabrudian: A case study from Qesem Cave, Israel , 2016 .

[26]  M W Marzke,et al.  A 3D quantitative comparison of trapezium and trapezoid relative articular and nonarticular surface areas in modern humans and great apes. , 2005, Journal of human evolution.

[27]  G. Dietrich,et al.  Skills and learning difficulties involved in stone knapping: The case of stone‐bead knapping in Khambhat, India , 1995 .

[28]  Phillip H. Shelley Variation in Lithic Assemblages: An Experiment , 1990 .

[29]  Ross Parry,et al.  Tool use ability depends on understanding of functional dynamics and not specific joint contribution profiles , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[30]  Blandine Bril,et al.  Coordination strategies used in stone knapping. , 2013, American journal of physical anthropology.

[31]  E. Biryukova,et al.  Organization of goal-directed action at a high level of motor skill: the case of stone knapping in India. , 2008, Motor control.

[32]  E. Trinkaus,et al.  Neandertal capitate-metacarpal articular morphology. , 1997, American journal of physical anthropology.

[33]  R. L. Linscheid,et al.  EMG study of hand muscle recruitment during hard hammer percussion manufacture of Oldowan tools. , 1998, American journal of physical anthropology.

[34]  N. Pigeot,et al.  Technical and Social Actors. Flintknapping Specialists and Apprentices at Magdalenian Etiolles , 1990 .

[35]  Metin I. Eren,et al.  Middle Paleolithic Skill Level and the Individual Knapper: An Experiment , 2011, American Antiquity.

[36]  Harold L. Dibble,et al.  Experimental Design and Experimental Inference in Stone Artifact Archaeology , 2018 .

[37]  B. Bril,et al.  How do stone knappers predict and control the outcome of flaking? Implications for understanding early stone tool technology. , 2010, Journal of human evolution.

[38]  Erin Marie Williams-Hatala,et al.  The manual pressures of stone tool behaviors and their implications for the evolution of the human hand. , 2018, Journal of human evolution.

[39]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Experimental studies illuminate the cultural transmission of percussive technologies in Homo and Pan , 2015, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  Alastair Key,et al.  Form and function in the Lower Palaeolithic: history, progress, and continued relevance. , 2017, Journal of anthropological sciences = Rivista di antropologia : JASS.

[41]  Brian G Richmond,et al.  Hand pressure distribution during Oldowan stone tool production. , 2012, Journal of human evolution.

[42]  S. Lycett,et al.  Toward a “Quantitative Genetic” Approach to Lithic Variation , 2015 .

[43]  R. L. Susman Hand of Paranthropus robustus from Member 1, Swartkrans: fossil evidence for tool behavior. , 1988, Science.

[44]  Carina Price,et al.  Validity and repeatability of three in-shoe pressure measurement systems. , 2016, Gait & posture.

[45]  Diane Gifford-Gonzalez,et al.  Bones are not enough: Analogues, knowledge, and interpretive strategies in zooarchaeology , 1991 .

[46]  R J Abboud,et al.  The Pedar in-shoe system: repeatability and normal pressure values. , 2007, Gait & posture.

[47]  Carlijn A. Vernooij,et al.  Learning to control orientation and force in a hammering task: The initial stage. , 2012 .

[48]  Christopher J Dunmore,et al.  The evolution of the hominin thumb and the influence exerted by the non-dominant hand during stone tool production. , 2015, Journal of human evolution.

[49]  M W Marzke,et al.  Precision grips, hand morphology, and tools. , 1997, American journal of physical anthropology.

[50]  J. Napier,et al.  Fossil Hand Bones from Olduvai Gorge , 1962, Nature.

[51]  Stephen J. Lycett,et al.  Built-in Misdirection: On the Difficulties of Learning to Knap , 2018, Lithic Technology.

[52]  M. Marzke,et al.  Hominid hand use in the pliocene and pleistocene: Evidence from experimental archaeology and comparative morphology* , 1986 .

[53]  A. Högberg,et al.  Lithic technology and behavioural modernity: new results from the Still Bay site, Hollow Rock Shelter, Western Cape Province, South Africa. , 2011, Journal of human evolution.

[54]  Chris Baber,et al.  Cognitive aspects of tool use. , 2006, Applied ergonomics.

[55]  Thomas G. McPoil,et al.  A comparison of two in-shoe plantar pressupe measurement systems , 1995 .

[56]  Erin Marie Williams-Hatala,et al.  Flake morphology as a record of manual pressure during stone tool production , 2017 .

[57]  Christopher J. Dunmore,et al.  The position of Australopithecus sediba within fossil hominin hand use diversity , 2020, Nature Ecology & Evolution.

[58]  D. Stout,et al.  Late Acheulean technology and cognition at Boxgrove, UK , 2014 .

[59]  A. Mateos,et al.  Shorter arms count: The energetic costs of raw material catchment in a new experimental approach at Sierra de Atapuerca , 2017 .

[60]  Blandine Bril,et al.  How similar are nut-cracking and stone-flaking? A functional approach to percussive technology , 2015, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[61]  Lionel Henry,et al.  A Grammar of Data Manipulation [R package dplyr version 1.0.2] , 2020 .

[62]  D. Schmitt,et al.  EMG of the human flexor pollicis longus muscle: implications for the evolution of hominid tool use. , 1998, Journal of human evolution.

[63]  Dorothy Fragaszy,et al.  Percussive tool use by Taï Western chimpanzees and Fazenda Boa Vista bearded capuchin monkeys: a comparison , 2015, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[64]  S. Milne Palaeo-Eskimo Novice Flintknapping in the Eastern Canadian Arctic , 2005 .

[65]  Frank Bretz,et al.  Simultaneous Inference in General Parametric Models [R package multcomp version 1.4-14] , 2020 .

[66]  B. Richmond,et al.  Biomechanical strategies for accuracy and force generation during stone tool production. , 2014, Journal of human evolution.

[67]  S. Coleman,et al.  Examining Movement Variability in the Basketball Free-Throw Action at Different Skill Levels , 2003, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[68]  Roger Bartlett,et al.  Three-Dimensional Evaluation of the Kinematic Release Parameters for Javelin Throwers of Different Skill Levels , 1996 .

[69]  Blandine Bril,et al.  Movement Pattern Variability in Stone Knapping: Implications for the Development of Percussive Traditions , 2014, PloS one.

[70]  E. Biryukova,et al.  Movement kinematics as an index of the level of motor skill: the case of Indian craftsmen stone knapping. , 2015, Motor control.

[71]  H. Kinoshita,et al.  Roles of proximal-to-distal sequential organization of the upper limb segments in striking the keys by expert pianists , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.

[72]  T. Chaminade,et al.  Neural correlates of Early Stone Age toolmaking: technology, language and cognition in human evolution , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[73]  M. Marzke,et al.  Evolution of the human hand: approaches to acquiring, analysing and interpreting the anatomical evidence , 2000, Journal of anatomy.