Benefits of repeated alternations - Task-specific vs. task-general sequential adjustments of dual-task order control.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Tilo Strobach,et al. Trial-to-trial modulation of task-order switch costs survive long inter-trial intervals , 2022, BMC psychology.
[2] Mareike A Hoffmann,et al. Task-order representations in dual tasks: Separate or integrated with component task sets? , 2021, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[3] T. Schubert,et al. A Gratton-like effect concerning task order in dual-task situations. , 2021, Acta psychologica.
[4] I. Koch,et al. Evidence for a multicomponent hierarchical representation of dual tasks , 2020, Memory & cognition.
[5] Tobias Egner,et al. Measuring Adaptive Control in Conflict Tasks , 2019, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[6] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Reconciling cognitive-control and episodic-retrieval accounts of sequential conflict modulation: Binding of control-states into event-files. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] Mareike A Hoffmann,et al. Motor sources of dual-task interference: Evidence for effector-based prioritization in dual-task control. , 2019, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.
[8] Lynn Huestegge,et al. Action scheduling in multitasking: A multi-phase framework of response-order control , 2019, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[9] Torsten Schubert,et al. Processing order in dual-task situations: The “first-come, first-served” principle and the impact of task order instructions , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[10] Andrea M Philipp,et al. Hierarchical task organization in dual tasks: evidence for higher level task representations , 2018, Psychological research.
[11] Christina B Reimer,et al. The impact of free-order and sequential-order instructions on task-order regulation in dual tasks , 2018, Psychological research.
[12] I. Koch,et al. Higher-Order Cognitive Control in Dual Tasks: Evidence From Task-Pair Switching , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] J. Grange,et al. The effect of N-3 on N-2 repetition costs in task switching. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[14] L. Huestegge,et al. Oculomotor dominance in multitasking: mechanisms of conflict resolution in cross-modal action. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[15] Lynn Huestegge,et al. Constraints in task-set control: modality dominance patterns among effector systems. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[16] H. Müller,et al. Temporal-order judgment of visual and auditory stimuli: modulations in situations with and without stimulus discrimination , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..
[17] R. Ulrich,et al. Processing two tasks with varying task order: central stage duration influences central processing order. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[18] Susana Ruiz Fernández,et al. Dual-task processing when task 1 is hard and task 2 is easy: reversed central processing order? , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] Andrea M Philipp,et al. The role of inhibition in task switching: A review , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[20] Torsten Schubert,et al. Task-order coordination in dual-task performance and the lateral prefrontal cortex: an event-related fMRI study , 2006, Psychological research.
[21] M. Sigman,et al. Dynamics of the Central Bottleneck: Dual-Task and Task Uncertainty , 2006, PLoS biology.
[22] N. Meiran,et al. Dual route for subtask order control: Evidence from the psychological refractory paradigm , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[23] Pierre Jolicoeur,et al. Testing the predictions of the central capacity sharing model. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] Roy Luria,et al. Online order control in the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] P. Jolicoeur,et al. A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] D. Navon,et al. Queuing or Sharing? A Critical Evaluation of the Single-Bottleneck Notion , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.
[27] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[28] R. Kliegl,et al. Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[29] B. Hommel. Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] David E. Kieras,et al. A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. , 1997 .
[31] R. Passingham. Attention to action. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[32] Ritske De Jong,et al. The Role of Preparation in Overlapping-task Performance , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[33] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[34] E. Donchin,et al. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[35] J. C. Johnston,et al. Chronometric Evidence for Central Postponement in Temporally Overlapping Tasks , 2003 .
[36] A. Welford. THE ‘PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD’ AND THE TIMING OF HIGH‐SPEED PERFORMANCE—A REVIEW AND A THEORY , 1952 .
[37] C. W. Telford. The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .