Benefits of repeated alternations - Task-specific vs. task-general sequential adjustments of dual-task order control.

[1]  Tilo Strobach,et al.  Trial-to-trial modulation of task-order switch costs survive long inter-trial intervals , 2022, BMC psychology.

[2]  Mareike A Hoffmann,et al.  Task-order representations in dual tasks: Separate or integrated with component task sets? , 2021, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  T. Schubert,et al.  A Gratton-like effect concerning task order in dual-task situations. , 2021, Acta psychologica.

[4]  I. Koch,et al.  Evidence for a multicomponent hierarchical representation of dual tasks , 2020, Memory & cognition.

[5]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Measuring Adaptive Control in Conflict Tasks , 2019, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Reconciling cognitive-control and episodic-retrieval accounts of sequential conflict modulation: Binding of control-states into event-files. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Mareike A Hoffmann,et al.  Motor sources of dual-task interference: Evidence for effector-based prioritization in dual-task control. , 2019, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

[8]  Lynn Huestegge,et al.  Action scheduling in multitasking: A multi-phase framework of response-order control , 2019, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[9]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Processing order in dual-task situations: The “first-come, first-served” principle and the impact of task order instructions , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[10]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Hierarchical task organization in dual tasks: evidence for higher level task representations , 2018, Psychological research.

[11]  Christina B Reimer,et al.  The impact of free-order and sequential-order instructions on task-order regulation in dual tasks , 2018, Psychological research.

[12]  I. Koch,et al.  Higher-Order Cognitive Control in Dual Tasks: Evidence From Task-Pair Switching , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  J. Grange,et al.  The effect of N-3 on N-2 repetition costs in task switching. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  L. Huestegge,et al.  Oculomotor dominance in multitasking: mechanisms of conflict resolution in cross-modal action. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[15]  Lynn Huestegge,et al.  Constraints in task-set control: modality dominance patterns among effector systems. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[16]  H. Müller,et al.  Temporal-order judgment of visual and auditory stimuli: modulations in situations with and without stimulus discrimination , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[17]  R. Ulrich,et al.  Processing two tasks with varying task order: central stage duration influences central processing order. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[18]  Susana Ruiz Fernández,et al.  Dual-task processing when task 1 is hard and task 2 is easy: reversed central processing order? , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  The role of inhibition in task switching: A review , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[20]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Task-order coordination in dual-task performance and the lateral prefrontal cortex: an event-related fMRI study , 2006, Psychological research.

[21]  M. Sigman,et al.  Dynamics of the Central Bottleneck: Dual-Task and Task Uncertainty , 2006, PLoS biology.

[22]  N. Meiran,et al.  Dual route for subtask order control: Evidence from the psychological refractory paradigm , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  Pierre Jolicoeur,et al.  Testing the predictions of the central capacity sharing model. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Roy Luria,et al.  Online order control in the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  D. Navon,et al.  Queuing or Sharing? A Critical Evaluation of the Single-Bottleneck Notion , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[28]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  B. Hommel Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  David E. Kieras,et al.  A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. , 1997 .

[31]  R. Passingham Attention to action. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[32]  Ritske De Jong,et al.  The Role of Preparation in Overlapping-task Performance , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[33]  H. Pashler Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[34]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[35]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Chronometric Evidence for Central Postponement in Temporally Overlapping Tasks , 2003 .

[36]  A. Welford THE ‘PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD’ AND THE TIMING OF HIGH‐SPEED PERFORMANCE—A REVIEW AND A THEORY , 1952 .

[37]  C. W. Telford The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .