Exploration Technique and Technical Innovations in Corvids and Parrots
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] S. Cobb. A Note on the Size of the Avian Olfactory Bulb , 1959, Epilepsia.
[2] A. Kamil,et al. of Nebraska - Lincoln of Nebraska - Lincoln Tool-Making and Tool-Using in the Northern Blue Jay Tool-Making and Tool-Using in the Northern Blue Jay , 2022 .
[3] P. Russell,et al. Relationships between exploratory behaviour and fear: a review. , 1973, British journal of psychology.
[4] S. Dudek. Creativity in Young Children - Attitude or Ability. , 1974 .
[5] H. Berkhoudt. The Morphology and Distribution of Cutaneous Mechanoreceptors (Herbst and Grandry Corpuscles) in Bill and Tongue of the Mallard (Anas Platyrhynchos L.) , 1979 .
[6] G. Nuechterlein,et al. Experiments on Olfactory Detection of Food Caches by Black-Billed Magpies , 1985 .
[7] A. Reiner. Is prefrontal cortex found only in mammals? , 1986, Trends in Neurosciences.
[8] A. Harriman,et al. Olfactory acuity in the common raven (Corvus corax) , 1986, Physiology & Behavior.
[9] Irene M. Pepperberg,et al. Object permanence in the African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) , 1986 .
[10] K. Connolly,et al. The Emergence of a Tool-Using Skill in Infancy. , 1989 .
[11] L. Harris,et al. Footedness in parrots: three centuries of research, theory, and mere surmise. , 1989, Canadian journal of psychology.
[12] E. Visalberghi,et al. Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). , 1994, Journal of comparative psychology.
[13] H. Berkhoudt,et al. Behavioral Mechanisms of Avian Feeding , 1994 .
[14] G. Hunt. Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows , 1996, Nature.
[15] I. Pepperberg,et al. Development of Piagetian Object Permanence in a Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus) , 1997 .
[16] L. Lefebvre,et al. Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds , 1997, Animal Behaviour.
[17] Walter J. Bock,et al. Functional and evolutionary morphology of woodpeckers , 1999 .
[19] G. Dyke,et al. A new psittaciform bird from the London Clay (Lower Eocene) of England , 2000 .
[20] J. Marzluff,et al. Cryptic genetic variation and paraphyly in ravens , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[21] L. Lefebvre,et al. Relative Size of the Hyperstriatum ventrale Is the Best Predictor of Feeding Innovation Rate in Birds , 2000, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.
[22] O. Güntürkün,et al. Development of object permanence in food-storing magpies (Pica pica) , 2000 .
[23] M. Koch,et al. Impairment in a discrimination reversal task after D1 receptor blockade in the pigeon "prefrontal cortex". , 2000, Behavioral neuroscience.
[24] R. Greenberg,et al. Ecological Aspects of Neophobia and Neophilia in Birds , 2001 .
[25] L. Huber,et al. Social learning affects object exploration and manipulation in keas, Nestor notabilis , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[26] Hideko Takeshita,et al. Development of combinatory manipulation in chimpanzee infants (Pan troglodytes) , 2001, Animal Cognition.
[27] L. Lefebvre,et al. Problem solving and neophobia in a columbiform–passeriform assemblage in Barbados , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[28] A. Kacelnik,et al. Shaping of Hooks in New Caledonian Crows , 2002, Science.
[29] Daniel Sol,et al. Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds , 2002, Animal Behaviour.
[30] M. Funk,et al. Problem solving skills in young yellow-crowned parakeets (Cyanoramphus auriceps) , 2002, Animal Cognition.
[31] H. Winkler,et al. The significance of ecological factors for exploration and neophobia in parrots , 2002 .
[32] K. Laland,et al. Animal innovation: An introduction. , 2003 .
[33] G. Martin,et al. Visual fields in hornbills: precision‐grasping and sunshades , 2003 .
[34] Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al. Cognitive development in object manipulation by infant chimpanzees , 2003, Animal Cognition.
[35] T. Roper. Olfactory discrimination in Yellow‐backed Chattering Lories Lorius garrulus flavopalliatus: first demonstration of olfaction in Psittaciformes , 2003 .
[36] R. Greenberg. The role of neophobia and neophilia in the development of innovative behaviour of birds , 2003 .
[37] L. Lefebvre,et al. Positive and negative correlates of feeding innovations in birds: evidence for limited modularity , 2003 .
[38] Nicola S. Clayton,et al. The Mentality of Crows: Convergent Evolution of Intelligence in Corvids and Apes , 2004, Science.
[39] L. Lefebvre,et al. Brains, Innovations and Evolution in Birds and Primates , 2004, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.
[40] E. Ottoni,et al. Preliminary observations of tool use in captive hyacinth macaws (Anodorhynchus hyacinthinus) , 2004, Animal Cognition.
[41] Andrew N. Iwaniuk,et al. Interspecific Allometry of the Brain and Brain Regions in Parrots (Psittaciformes): Comparisons with Other Birds and Primates , 2004, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.
[42] K. Gottschaldt,et al. The peripheral morphological basis of tactile sensibility in the beak of geese , 2004, Cell and Tissue Research.
[43] Dorothy Fragaszy,et al. Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools , 2004, American journal of primatology.
[44] N. Emery. Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[45] B. Heinrich,et al. Testing Problem Solving in Ravens: String-Pulling to Reach Food , 2005 .
[46] T. Matsuzawa,et al. Cognitive Development in Apes and Humans Assessed by Object Manipulation , 2006 .
[47] T. Torigoe. Comparison of object manipulation among 74 species of non-human primates , 1985, Primates.
[48] N. Emery,et al. Investigating Physical Cognition in Rooks, Corvus frugilegus , 2006, Current Biology.
[49] Nicola S. Clayton,et al. Non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus, solve the trap-tube problem , 2007, Animal Cognition.
[50] Ludwig Huber,et al. A case of quick problem solving in birds: string pulling in keas, Nestor notabilis , 2006, Animal Behaviour.
[51] L. Huber,et al. Technical intelligence in animals: the kea model , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[52] G. Martin,et al. Vision and the foraging technique of skimmers (Rynchopidae) , 2007 .
[53] G. Martin. Visual fields and their functions in birds , 2007, Journal of Ornithology.
[54] Alex H. Taylor,et al. Spontaneous Metatool Use by New Caledonian Crows , 2007, Current Biology.
[55] Mike Hansell,et al. Setting tool use within the context of animal construction behaviour. , 2008, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[56] N. Emery,et al. Cooperative problem solving in rooks (Corvus frugilegus) , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[57] P. Gunz,et al. Functional morphology and integration of corvid skulls – a 3D geometric morphometric approach , 2009, Frontiers in Zoology.
[58] Cynthia Schuck-Paim,et al. Means to an end: Neotropical parrots manage to pull strings to meet their goals , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[59] A. H. Taylor,et al. Do New Caledonian crows solve physical problems through causal reasoning? , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[60] E. Ottoni,et al. Learning generalization in problem solving by a blue-fronted parrot (Amazona aestiva) , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[61] G. Smith. SYSTEMATICS OF PARROTS , 2008 .
[62] R. Heathcote,et al. The Role of Experience in Problem Solving and Innovative Tool Use in Crows , 2009, Current Biology.
[63] A. Kacelnik,et al. Cognitive Processes Associated with Sequential Tool Use in New Caledonian Crows , 2009, PloS one.
[64] Culum Brown,et al. Laterality enhances cognition in Australian parrots , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[65] Ludwig Huber,et al. Kea (Nestor notabilis) consider spatial relationships between objects in the support problem , 2009, Biology Letters.
[66] Christopher D. Bird,et al. Rooks Use Stones to Raise the Water Level to Reach a Floating Worm , 2009, Current Biology.
[67] Nathan J Emery,et al. Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool-using rooks , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[68] T. Bugnyar,et al. What You See Is What You Get? Exclusion Performances in Ravens and Keas , 2009, PLoS ONE.
[69] J. Kaufman,et al. Beyond Big and Little: The Four C Model of Creativity , 2009 .
[70] M. Cunningham,et al. Bill Morphology of Ibises Suggests a Remote-Tactile Sensory System for Prey Detection , 2010 .
[71] I. Teschke,et al. The tale of the finch: adaptive radiation and behavioural flexibility , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[72] Kazuo Fujita,et al. How do keas (Nestor notabilis) solve artificial-fruit problems with multiple locks? , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[73] Christian Rutz,et al. The Ecological Significance of Tool Use in New Caledonian Crows , 2010, Science.
[74] G. Burghardt,et al. Current Perspectives on the Biological Study of Play: Signs of Progress , 2010, The Quarterly Review of Biology.
[75] Alex H. Taylor,et al. Complex cognition and behavioural innovation in New Caledonian crows , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[76] L. Huber,et al. Kea, Nestor notabilis, produce dynamic relationships between objects in a second-order tool use task , 2010, Animal Behaviour.
[77] L. Huber,et al. Big brains are not enough: performance of three parrot species in the trap-tube paradigm , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[78] Zoe P. Demery,et al. Vision, touch and object manipulation in Senegal parrots Poicephalus senegalus , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[79] Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al. How does cognition evolve? Phylogenetic comparative psychology , 2012, Animal Cognition.
[80] Nicola S. Clayton,et al. Tool-use and instrumental learning in the Eurasian jay (Garrulus glandarius) , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[81] Culum Brown,et al. Cerebral lateralization determines hand preferences in Australian parrots , 2011, Biology Letters.
[82] L. Huber,et al. Navigating a tool end in a specific direction: stick-tool use in kea (Nestor notabilis) , 2011, Biology Letters.
[83] A. Kacelnik,et al. On the evolutionary and ontogenetic origins of tool-oriented behaviour in New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides). , 2011, Biological journal of the Linnean Society. Linnean Society of London.
[84] I. Teschke,et al. Physical cognition and tool-use: performance of Darwin’s finches in the two-trap tube task , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[85] A. Kacelnik,et al. Flexibility in Problem Solving and Tool Use of Kea and New Caledonian Crows in a Multi Access Box Paradigm , 2011, PloS one.
[86] L. Huber,et al. Keas rely on social information in a tool use task but abandon it in favour of overt exploration , 2011 .
[87] A. Nieder,et al. Ontogeny of object permanence and object tracking in the carrion crow, Corvus corone , 2011, Animal Behaviour.
[88] A. Kacelnik,et al. Spontaneous innovation in tool manufacture and use in a Goffin’s cockatoo , 2012, Current Biology.
[89] C. Schloegl,et al. Grey parrots use inferential reasoning based on acoustic cues alone , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[90] Jacqueline Fagard,et al. The emergence of tool use during the second year of life. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[91] Jolyon Troscianko,et al. Extreme binocular vision and a straight bill facilitate tool use in New Caledonian crows , 2012, Nature Communications.
[92] Alice M.I. Auersperg,et al. A new approach to comparing problem solving, flexibility and innovation , 2012, Communicative & integrative biology.
[93] D. Brunton,et al. Olfactory sensitivity in Kea and Kaka , 2012 .
[94] Joshua M. Plotnik,et al. Exclusion in corvids: the performance of food-caching Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius). , 2013, Journal of comparative psychology.
[95] G. Hunt,et al. Why is tool use rare in animals , 2013 .
[96] Toni Vernelli. The Complexity of Neophobia in a Generalist Foraging Corvid: the common magpie (Pica pica) , 2013 .
[97] Alex Kacelnik,et al. Explorative Learning and Functional Inferences on a Five-Step Means-Means-End Problem in Goffin’s Cockatoos ( Cacatua goffini ) , 2013, PloS one.
[98] Vincent M Janik,et al. Bottlenose dolphins can use learned vocal labels to address each other , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[99] J. Call. Three ingredients for becoming a creative tool user , 2013 .
[100] L. Huber,et al. What a parrot's mind adds to play: the urge to produce novelty fosters tool use acquisition in kea. , 2014 .
[101] H. Harley,et al. The role of signature whistle matching in bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus , 2014, Animal Behaviour.
[102] Roger E. Beaty,et al. Does insight problem solving predict real-world creativity? , 2014 .
[103] T. Bugnyar,et al. Unrewarded Object Combinations in Captive Parrots , 2014, Animal behavior and cognition.
[104] A. Kacelnik,et al. Combinatory actions during object play in psittaciformes (Diopsittaca nobilis, Pionites melanocephala, Cacatua goffini) and corvids (Corvus corax, C. monedula, C. moneduloides). , 2015, Journal of comparative psychology.