Exploration Technique and Technical Innovations in Corvids and Parrots

[1]  S. Cobb A Note on the Size of the Avian Olfactory Bulb , 1959, Epilepsia.

[2]  A. Kamil,et al.  of Nebraska - Lincoln of Nebraska - Lincoln Tool-Making and Tool-Using in the Northern Blue Jay Tool-Making and Tool-Using in the Northern Blue Jay , 2022 .

[3]  P. Russell,et al.  Relationships between exploratory behaviour and fear: a review. , 1973, British journal of psychology.

[4]  S. Dudek Creativity in Young Children - Attitude or Ability. , 1974 .

[5]  H. Berkhoudt The Morphology and Distribution of Cutaneous Mechanoreceptors (Herbst and Grandry Corpuscles) in Bill and Tongue of the Mallard (Anas Platyrhynchos L.) , 1979 .

[6]  G. Nuechterlein,et al.  Experiments on Olfactory Detection of Food Caches by Black-Billed Magpies , 1985 .

[7]  A. Reiner Is prefrontal cortex found only in mammals? , 1986, Trends in Neurosciences.

[8]  A. Harriman,et al.  Olfactory acuity in the common raven (Corvus corax) , 1986, Physiology & Behavior.

[9]  Irene M. Pepperberg,et al.  Object permanence in the African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) , 1986 .

[10]  K. Connolly,et al.  The Emergence of a Tool-Using Skill in Infancy. , 1989 .

[11]  L. Harris,et al.  Footedness in parrots: three centuries of research, theory, and mere surmise. , 1989, Canadian journal of psychology.

[12]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). , 1994, Journal of comparative psychology.

[13]  H. Berkhoudt,et al.  Behavioral Mechanisms of Avian Feeding , 1994 .

[14]  G. Hunt Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows , 1996, Nature.

[15]  I. Pepperberg,et al.  Development of Piagetian Object Permanence in a Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus) , 1997 .

[16]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  Walter J. Bock,et al.  Functional and evolutionary morphology of woodpeckers , 1999 .

[18]  Metallothionein-a cDna in Alpine Arctic Char (Salvelinus Alpinus): Notes On Metal Tolerance and Relationships To Other Salmonids1 , 2000 .

[19]  G. Dyke,et al.  A new psittaciform bird from the London Clay (Lower Eocene) of England , 2000 .

[20]  J. Marzluff,et al.  Cryptic genetic variation and paraphyly in ravens , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Relative Size of the Hyperstriatum ventrale Is the Best Predictor of Feeding Innovation Rate in Birds , 2000, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[22]  O. Güntürkün,et al.  Development of object permanence in food-storing magpies (Pica pica) , 2000 .

[23]  M. Koch,et al.  Impairment in a discrimination reversal task after D1 receptor blockade in the pigeon "prefrontal cortex". , 2000, Behavioral neuroscience.

[24]  R. Greenberg,et al.  Ecological Aspects of Neophobia and Neophilia in Birds , 2001 .

[25]  L. Huber,et al.  Social learning affects object exploration and manipulation in keas, Nestor notabilis , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  Hideko Takeshita,et al.  Development of combinatory manipulation in chimpanzee infants (Pan troglodytes) , 2001, Animal Cognition.

[27]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Problem solving and neophobia in a columbiform–passeriform assemblage in Barbados , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[28]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Shaping of Hooks in New Caledonian Crows , 2002, Science.

[29]  Daniel Sol,et al.  Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  M. Funk,et al.  Problem solving skills in young yellow-crowned parakeets (Cyanoramphus auriceps) , 2002, Animal Cognition.

[31]  H. Winkler,et al.  The significance of ecological factors for exploration and neophobia in parrots , 2002 .

[32]  K. Laland,et al.  Animal innovation: An introduction. , 2003 .

[33]  G. Martin,et al.  Visual fields in hornbills: precision‐grasping and sunshades , 2003 .

[34]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Cognitive development in object manipulation by infant chimpanzees , 2003, Animal Cognition.

[35]  T. Roper Olfactory discrimination in Yellow‐backed Chattering Lories Lorius garrulus flavopalliatus: first demonstration of olfaction in Psittaciformes , 2003 .

[36]  R. Greenberg The role of neophobia and neophilia in the development of innovative behaviour of birds , 2003 .

[37]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Positive and negative correlates of feeding innovations in birds: evidence for limited modularity , 2003 .

[38]  Nicola S. Clayton,et al.  The Mentality of Crows: Convergent Evolution of Intelligence in Corvids and Apes , 2004, Science.

[39]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Brains, Innovations and Evolution in Birds and Primates , 2004, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[40]  E. Ottoni,et al.  Preliminary observations of tool use in captive hyacinth macaws (Anodorhynchus hyacinthinus) , 2004, Animal Cognition.

[41]  Andrew N. Iwaniuk,et al.  Interspecific Allometry of the Brain and Brain Regions in Parrots (Psittaciformes): Comparisons with Other Birds and Primates , 2004, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[42]  K. Gottschaldt,et al.  The peripheral morphological basis of tactile sensibility in the beak of geese , 2004, Cell and Tissue Research.

[43]  Dorothy Fragaszy,et al.  Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools , 2004, American journal of primatology.

[44]  N. Emery Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Testing Problem Solving in Ravens: String-Pulling to Reach Food , 2005 .

[46]  T. Matsuzawa,et al.  Cognitive Development in Apes and Humans Assessed by Object Manipulation , 2006 .

[47]  T. Torigoe Comparison of object manipulation among 74 species of non-human primates , 1985, Primates.

[48]  N. Emery,et al.  Investigating Physical Cognition in Rooks, Corvus frugilegus , 2006, Current Biology.

[49]  Nicola S. Clayton,et al.  Non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus, solve the trap-tube problem , 2007, Animal Cognition.

[50]  Ludwig Huber,et al.  A case of quick problem solving in birds: string pulling in keas, Nestor notabilis , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[51]  L. Huber,et al.  Technical intelligence in animals: the kea model , 2006, Animal Cognition.

[52]  G. Martin,et al.  Vision and the foraging technique of skimmers (Rynchopidae) , 2007 .

[53]  G. Martin Visual fields and their functions in birds , 2007, Journal of Ornithology.

[54]  Alex H. Taylor,et al.  Spontaneous Metatool Use by New Caledonian Crows , 2007, Current Biology.

[55]  Mike Hansell,et al.  Setting tool use within the context of animal construction behaviour. , 2008, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[56]  N. Emery,et al.  Cooperative problem solving in rooks (Corvus frugilegus) , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[57]  P. Gunz,et al.  Functional morphology and integration of corvid skulls – a 3D geometric morphometric approach , 2009, Frontiers in Zoology.

[58]  Cynthia Schuck-Paim,et al.  Means to an end: Neotropical parrots manage to pull strings to meet their goals , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[59]  A. H. Taylor,et al.  Do New Caledonian crows solve physical problems through causal reasoning? , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[60]  E. Ottoni,et al.  Learning generalization in problem solving by a blue-fronted parrot (Amazona aestiva) , 2008, Animal Cognition.

[61]  G. Smith SYSTEMATICS OF PARROTS , 2008 .

[62]  R. Heathcote,et al.  The Role of Experience in Problem Solving and Innovative Tool Use in Crows , 2009, Current Biology.

[63]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Cognitive Processes Associated with Sequential Tool Use in New Caledonian Crows , 2009, PloS one.

[64]  Culum Brown,et al.  Laterality enhances cognition in Australian parrots , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[65]  Ludwig Huber,et al.  Kea (Nestor notabilis) consider spatial relationships between objects in the support problem , 2009, Biology Letters.

[66]  Christopher D. Bird,et al.  Rooks Use Stones to Raise the Water Level to Reach a Floating Worm , 2009, Current Biology.

[67]  Nathan J Emery,et al.  Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool-using rooks , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[68]  T. Bugnyar,et al.  What You See Is What You Get? Exclusion Performances in Ravens and Keas , 2009, PLoS ONE.

[69]  J. Kaufman,et al.  Beyond Big and Little: The Four C Model of Creativity , 2009 .

[70]  M. Cunningham,et al.  Bill Morphology of Ibises Suggests a Remote-Tactile Sensory System for Prey Detection , 2010 .

[71]  I. Teschke,et al.  The tale of the finch: adaptive radiation and behavioural flexibility , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[72]  Kazuo Fujita,et al.  How do keas (Nestor notabilis) solve artificial-fruit problems with multiple locks? , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[73]  Christian Rutz,et al.  The Ecological Significance of Tool Use in New Caledonian Crows , 2010, Science.

[74]  G. Burghardt,et al.  Current Perspectives on the Biological Study of Play: Signs of Progress , 2010, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[75]  Alex H. Taylor,et al.  Complex cognition and behavioural innovation in New Caledonian crows , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[76]  L. Huber,et al.  Kea, Nestor notabilis, produce dynamic relationships between objects in a second-order tool use task , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[77]  L. Huber,et al.  Big brains are not enough: performance of three parrot species in the trap-tube paradigm , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[78]  Zoe P. Demery,et al.  Vision, touch and object manipulation in Senegal parrots Poicephalus senegalus , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[79]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  How does cognition evolve? Phylogenetic comparative psychology , 2012, Animal Cognition.

[80]  Nicola S. Clayton,et al.  Tool-use and instrumental learning in the Eurasian jay (Garrulus glandarius) , 2011, Animal Cognition.

[81]  Culum Brown,et al.  Cerebral lateralization determines hand preferences in Australian parrots , 2011, Biology Letters.

[82]  L. Huber,et al.  Navigating a tool end in a specific direction: stick-tool use in kea (Nestor notabilis) , 2011, Biology Letters.

[83]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  On the evolutionary and ontogenetic origins of tool-oriented behaviour in New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides). , 2011, Biological journal of the Linnean Society. Linnean Society of London.

[84]  I. Teschke,et al.  Physical cognition and tool-use: performance of Darwin’s finches in the two-trap tube task , 2011, Animal Cognition.

[85]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Flexibility in Problem Solving and Tool Use of Kea and New Caledonian Crows in a Multi Access Box Paradigm , 2011, PloS one.

[86]  L. Huber,et al.  Keas rely on social information in a tool use task but abandon it in favour of overt exploration , 2011 .

[87]  A. Nieder,et al.  Ontogeny of object permanence and object tracking in the carrion crow, Corvus corone , 2011, Animal Behaviour.

[88]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Spontaneous innovation in tool manufacture and use in a Goffin’s cockatoo , 2012, Current Biology.

[89]  C. Schloegl,et al.  Grey parrots use inferential reasoning based on acoustic cues alone , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[90]  Jacqueline Fagard,et al.  The emergence of tool use during the second year of life. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[91]  Jolyon Troscianko,et al.  Extreme binocular vision and a straight bill facilitate tool use in New Caledonian crows , 2012, Nature Communications.

[92]  Alice M.I. Auersperg,et al.  A new approach to comparing problem solving, flexibility and innovation , 2012, Communicative & integrative biology.

[93]  D. Brunton,et al.  Olfactory sensitivity in Kea and Kaka , 2012 .

[94]  Joshua M. Plotnik,et al.  Exclusion in corvids: the performance of food-caching Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius). , 2013, Journal of comparative psychology.

[95]  G. Hunt,et al.  Why is tool use rare in animals , 2013 .

[96]  Toni Vernelli The Complexity of Neophobia in a Generalist Foraging Corvid: the common magpie (Pica pica) , 2013 .

[97]  Alex Kacelnik,et al.  Explorative Learning and Functional Inferences on a Five-Step Means-Means-End Problem in Goffin’s Cockatoos ( Cacatua goffini ) , 2013, PloS one.

[98]  Vincent M Janik,et al.  Bottlenose dolphins can use learned vocal labels to address each other , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[99]  J. Call Three ingredients for becoming a creative tool user , 2013 .

[100]  L. Huber,et al.  What a parrot's mind adds to play: the urge to produce novelty fosters tool use acquisition in kea. , 2014 .

[101]  H. Harley,et al.  The role of signature whistle matching in bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus , 2014, Animal Behaviour.

[102]  Roger E. Beaty,et al.  Does insight problem solving predict real-world creativity? , 2014 .

[103]  T. Bugnyar,et al.  Unrewarded Object Combinations in Captive Parrots , 2014, Animal behavior and cognition.

[104]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Combinatory actions during object play in psittaciformes (Diopsittaca nobilis, Pionites melanocephala, Cacatua goffini) and corvids (Corvus corax, C. monedula, C. moneduloides). , 2015, Journal of comparative psychology.