Concreteness and word production
暂无分享,去创建一个
J Richard Hanley | Rebecka P Hunt | Deborah A Steed | Shannon Jackman | Rebecka P. Hunt | Deborah A. Steed | Shannon Jackman | J. Richard Hanley
[1] Janet Metcalfe,et al. Tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states: retrieval, behavior, and experience , 2011, Memory & cognition.
[2] A. Paivio. Mental Representations: A Dual Coding Approach , 1986 .
[3] Alan S. Brown,et al. From tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) data to theoretical implications in two steps: when more TOTs means better retrieval. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[4] Kevin Dent,et al. British-English norms and naming times for a set of 539 pictures: The role of age of acquisition , 2010, Behavior research methods.
[5] J. Hanley,et al. Are Reading and Spelling Phonologically Mediated? Evidence from a Patient with a Speech Production Impairment , 1997 .
[6] C. Hulme,et al. Speech and language processing mechanisms in verbal serial recall , 2006 .
[7] J. Kay,et al. An Effect of Imageability on the Production of Phonological Errors in Auditory Repetition , 1997 .
[8] Ken McRae,et al. Category - Specific semantic deficits , 2008 .
[9] Meredith A. Shafto,et al. Aging and Language Production , 2004, Current directions in psychological science.
[10] E. Warrington,et al. Abstract and concrete concepts have structurally different representational frameworks. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[11] J. Hanley,et al. The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: Do experimenter-presented interlopers have any effect? , 1992, Cognition.
[12] T. Jaeger,et al. Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.
[13] S. Crutch,et al. Contrasting Graded Effects of Semantic Similarity and Association across the Concreteness Spectrum , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[14] K Bock,et al. The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: Blocking or partial activation? , 1992, Memory & cognition.
[15] T. Shallice,et al. Deep Dyslexia: A Case Study of , 1993 .
[16] Gregory V. Jones. Deep dyslexia, imageability, and ease of predication , 1985, Brain and Language.
[17] G. Dell,et al. Evidence for the involvement of a nonlexical route in the repetition of familiar words: A comparison of single and dual route models of auditory repetition , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[18] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[19] G. Dell,et al. Models of Impaired Lexical Access in Speech Production , 2000 .
[20] E. Shoben,et al. Differential Context Effects in the Comprehension of Abstract and Concrete Verbal Materials , 1983 .
[21] J. Hanley. Why are names of people associated with so many phonological retrieval failures? , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[22] Daniel Mirman,et al. Attractor Dynamics and Semantic Neighborhood Density: Processing Is Slowed by near Neighbors and Speeded by Distant Neighbors We Thank Ann Kulikowski for Her Help with Data Collection And , 2022 .
[23] B. Ober,et al. Naturally occurring and experimentally induced tip-of-the-tongue experiences in three adult age groups. , 1999, Psychology and aging.
[24] T. Bormann,et al. Omissions and semantic errors in aphasic naming: Is there a link? , 2008, Brain and Language.
[25] G. Dell,et al. Models of errors of omission in aphasic naming , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[26] G. Dell,et al. Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.
[27] Audrey K. Kittredge,et al. Where is the effect of frequency in word production? Insights from aphasic picture-naming errors , 2008, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[28] E. Warrington,et al. The different representational frameworks underpinning abstract and concrete knowledge: Evidence from odd-one-out judgements , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[29] Matti Laine,et al. Coherence and informativeness of discourse in two dementia types , 1998, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[30] Matthew A Goldrick,et al. Limited interaction in speech production: Chronometric, speech error, and neuropsychological evidence , 2006 .
[31] J. Richard Hanley,et al. Why are Familiar-Only Experiences More Frequent for Voices than for Faces? , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[32] Laine Matti,et al. Modelling anomia by the discrete two-stage word production architecture , 1998, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[33] Concreteness Effects in Word Production but Not Word Comprehension in Deep Dyslexia , 1997 .
[34] Alan S. Brown. The Tip of the Tongue State , 2011 .
[35] Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al. Semantic Diversity Accounts for the “Missing” Word Frequency Effect in Stroke Aphasia: Insights Using a Novel Method to Quantify Contextual Variability in Meaning , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.