Journal of Occupational Medicine and Toxicology Comfort in Big Numbers: Does Over-estimation of Doping Prevalence in Others Indicate Self-involvement?

BackgroundThe 'False Consensus Effect' (FCE), by which people perceive their own actions as relatively common behaviour, might be exploited to gauge whether a person engages in controversial behaviour, such as performance enhancing drug (PED) use.HypothesisIt is assumed that people's own behaviour, owing to the FCE, affects their estimation of the prevalence of that behaviour. It is further hypothesised that a person's estimate of PED population use is a reliable indicator of the doping behaviour of that person, in lieu of self-reports.Testing the hypothesisOver- or underestimation is calculated from investigating known groups (i.e. users vs. non-users), using a short questionnaire, and a known prevalence rate from official reports or sample evidence. It is proposed that sample evidence from self-reported behaviour should be verified using objective biochemical analyses.In order to find proofs of concept for the existence of false consensus, a pilot study was conducted. Data were collected among competitive UK student-athletes (n = 124) using a web-based anonymous questionnaire. User (n = 9) vs. non-user (n = 76) groups were established using self-reported information on doping use and intention to use PEDs in hypothetical situations. Observed differences in the mean estimation of doping made by the user group exceeded the estimation made by the non-user group (35.11% vs. 15.34% for general doping and 34.25% vs. 26.30% in hypothetical situations, respectively), thus providing preliminary evidence in support of the FCE concept in relation to doping.Implications of the hypothesisThe presence of the FCE in estimating doping prevalence or behaviour in others suggests that the FCE based approach may be an avenue for developing an indirect self-report mechanism for PED use behaviour. The method may be successfully adapted to the estimation of prevalence of behaviours where direct self-reports are assumed to be distorted by socially desirable responding. Thus this method can enhance available information on socially undesirable, health compromising behaviour (i.e. PED use) for policy makers and healthcare professionals. The importance of the method lies in its usefulness in epidemiological studies, not in individual assessments.

[1]  H. Alaranta,et al.  Self-Reported Attitudes of Elite Athletes Towards Doping: Differences Between Type of Sport , 2006, International journal of sports medicine.

[2]  D. S. Holmes,et al.  Dimensions of projection. , 1968, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  B. Ludes,et al.  Hair analysis of seven bodybuilders for anabolic steroids, ephedrine, and clenbuterol. , 2002, Journal of forensic sciences.

[4]  F. Siero,et al.  The double-edged sword of providing information about the prevalence of safer sex , 2002 .

[5]  W. Morgan Fair is Fair, Or Is It?: A Moral Consideration of the Doping Wars in American Sport , 2006, Doping in Sport.

[6]  K. Dietz,et al.  Doping in fitness sports: estimated number of unreported cases and individual probability of doping. , 2006, Addiction.

[7]  T. Lam,et al.  Perceived peer smoking prevalence and its association with smoking behaviours and intentions in Hong Kong Chinese adolescents. , 2004, Addiction.

[8]  Michael I Norton,et al.  Perceptions of a Fluid Consensus: Uniqueness Bias, False Consensus, False Polarization, and Pluralistic Ignorance in a Water Conservation Crisis , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[9]  I. Waddington Changing Patterns of Drug Use in British Sport from the 1960s , 2005 .

[10]  J. Parkkari,et al.  Doping Use among Tertiary Education Students in Six Developed Countries , 2006, European Journal of Epidemiology.

[11]  Peter G. M. van der Heijden,et al.  Meta-Analysis of Randomized Response Research , 2005 .

[12]  G. Sancesario,et al.  Calcium, copper, iron, magnesium, silicon and zinc content of hair in Parkinson's disease. , 2005, Journal of trace elements in medicine and biology : organ of the Society for Minerals and Trace Elements.

[13]  J. Cunningham,et al.  Implications of the normative fallacy in young adult smokers aged 19-24 years. , 2007, American journal of public health.

[14]  D. Seal,et al.  Social Comparisons of One's Own With Others' Attitudes Toward Casual and Responsible Sex1 , 1998 .

[15]  T. Balaji,et al.  Determination of iron, cobalt, nickel, manganese, zinc, copper, cadmium and lead in human hair by inductively coupled plasma-atomic emission spectrometry , 2002 .

[16]  L. Ross,et al.  The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes , 1977 .

[17]  R. DiClemente,et al.  Sexual behaviors and their correlates among young people in Mauritius: a cross-sectional study , 2007, BMC international health and human rights.

[18]  J. Krueger,et al.  The truly false consensus effect: an ineradicable and egocentric bias in social perception. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  P. Laure Epidemiologic approach of doping in sport. A review. , 1997, The Journal of sports medicine and physical fitness.

[20]  B. Kayser,et al.  Current anti-doping policy: a critical appraisal , 2007, BMC medical ethics.

[21]  A. Petróczi,et al.  Performance enhancement with supplements: incongruence between rationale and practice , 2007, Journal of the International Society of Sports Nutrition.

[22]  A. Petróczi,et al.  Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy Psychological Drivers in Doping: the Life-cycle Model of Performance Enhancement , 2007 .

[23]  P. Kintz,et al.  Evaluation of the IDS One-Step ELISA kits for the detection of illicit drugs in hair. , 2007, Forensic science international.

[24]  Steven C Martino,et al.  "But Others Do It!": Do Misperceptions of Schoolmate Alcohol and Marijuana Use Predict Subsequent Drug Use Among Young Adolescents? , 2007 .

[25]  F. Siero,et al.  The Division of Labor Among Egalitarian and Traditional Women: Differences in Discontent, Social Comparison, and False Consensus , 2000 .

[26]  S. McCabe Misperceptions of non-medical prescription drug use: a web survey of college students. , 2008, Addictive behaviors.

[27]  R. Kazlauskas,et al.  Sports drug testing--an analyst's perspective. , 2004, Chemical Society reviews.

[28]  S Wolfson Students' estimates of the prevalence of drug use: evidence for a false consensus effect. , 2000, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[29]  C. Ellertson,et al.  The Measure of Induced Abortion Levels in Mexico Using Random Response Technique , 2006 .

[30]  Jerry Suls,et al.  False Consensus and False Uniqueness in Estimating the Prevalence of Health‐Protective Behaviors , 1988 .

[31]  K. Anwer,et al.  Age- and sex-based comparative distribution of selected metals in the scalp hair of an urban population from two cities in Pakistan. , 1995, Environmental pollution.

[32]  Ashesh Mukherjee,et al.  What's Not to Like? Preference Asymmetry in the False Consensus Effect , 2008 .

[33]  A. Petróczi,et al.  Limited agreement exists between rationale and practice in athletes' supplement use for maintenance of health: a retrospective study , 2007, Nutrition journal.

[34]  F. Pragst,et al.  State of the art in hair analysis for detection of drug and alcohol abuse. , 2006, Clinica chimica acta; international journal of clinical chemistry.