Reality compared with its alternatives: age differences in judgments of regret and relief.

Three experiments examined developmental change in children's understanding of regret and relief, two second-order emotions whose quality depends on a comparison between reality and "what might have been." In Experiment 1, participants 7 years of age and older, but not 5-year-olds, made regret-related emotion-response judgments that took into account a comparison of reality with its alternatives. In Experiment 2, 5-year-olds judged that an individual would feel better, rather than worse, when a counterfactual outcome was better than what actually occurred (the opposite of the pattern found with older children and adults). Experiment 3 focused on the understanding of relief. In contrast to the findings from Experiment I, the 7-year-olds in Experiment 3 made their judgments solely on the basis of what actually occurred.

[1]  S. Denham Emotional development in young children , 1998 .

[2]  Tim P. German Children’s causal reasoning: Counterfactual thinking occurs for ‘negative’ outcomes only , 1999 .

[3]  M. Zeelenberg Anticipated regret, expected feedback and behavioral decision-making , 1999 .

[4]  James M. Olson,et al.  Counterfactual thinking: A critical overview. , 1995 .

[5]  S. Denham,et al.  Young preschoolers' understanding of emotions. , 1990 .

[6]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Counterfactual Thought, Regret, and Superstition: How to Avoid Kicking Yourself , 1995 .

[7]  N. Roese,et al.  What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking , 1995 .

[8]  B. Mellers Choice and the relative pleasure of consequences. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  Steven J. Sherman,et al.  The Role of Counterfactual Thinking in Judgments of Affect , 1990 .

[10]  N. Roese The Functional Basis of Counterfactual Thinking , 1994 .

[11]  David F. Bjorklund,et al.  Children's strategies: Contemporary views of cognitive development. , 1991 .

[12]  N. Roese,et al.  Impoverished Counterfactual Thinking is Associated with Schizophrenia , 2000, Psychiatry.

[13]  S. Kuczaj,et al.  The development of hypothetical reference in the speech of young children , 1979, Journal of Child Language.

[14]  Paul L. Harris,et al.  Children's understanding of emotion , 1989 .

[15]  S. Denham Emotional Development in Young Children (The Guilford Series on Social and Emotional Development) , 1998 .

[16]  M. Bar-Hillel,et al.  Why are people reluctant to exchange lottery tickets , 1996 .

[17]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  Young Children's Appraisals of Others' Spontaneous Emotional Reactions. , 1991 .

[18]  Children's conceptions of sociomoral affect: Happy victimizers, mixed emotions, and other expectancies. , 1995 .

[19]  R. Kail,et al.  Information processing and cognitive development. , 1982, Advances in child development and behavior.

[20]  D. Hart,et al.  Morality in everyday life : developmental perspectives , 1995 .

[21]  N. Roese,et al.  Affective Determinants of Counterfactual Thinking , 1997 .

[22]  I. Ritov,et al.  Probability of Regret: Anticipation of Uncertainty Resolution in Choice , 1996 .

[23]  R. Kail,et al.  Processing Time Declines Exponentially during Childhood and Adolescence. , 1991 .

[24]  N. Roese Counterfactual thinking. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  R. Guttentag,et al.  The mental effort requirement of cumulative rehearsal: A developmental study. , 1984 .

[26]  Richard Wiseman,et al.  The Luck Factor , 2003 .

[27]  L. Frank The Society for Research in Child Development , 1935 .

[28]  Susanne A. Denham,et al.  Social Cognition, Prosocial Behavior and Emotion in Preschoolers: Contextual Validation. , 1986 .

[29]  J. Strayer Children's Attributions Regarding the Situational Determinants of Emotion in Self and Others. , 1986 .

[30]  A. Tversky,et al.  The Psychology of Preferences , 1982 .

[31]  J. Baron,et al.  Psychological determinants of decision attitude , 1994 .

[32]  A. Manstead,et al.  On bad decisions and disconfirmed expectancies: The psychology of regret and disappointment , 2000 .

[33]  P. Harris,et al.  Children's use of counterfactual thinking in causal reasoning , 1996, Cognition.

[34]  Children's understanding of moral emotions. , 1988 .

[35]  S. Harter The Perceived Competence Scale for Children. , 1982 .

[36]  A. Berti,et al.  The understanding of sadness, guilt, and shame in 5-, 7-, and 9-year-old children. , 2000, Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs.

[37]  S. Harter A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the classroom: Motivational and informational components. , 1981 .

[38]  R. Case,et al.  Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span , 1982 .

[39]  C. Anderson The Psychology of Doing Nothing: Forms of Decision Avoidance Result from Reason and Emotion , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  Mark Meerum Terwogt,et al.  Children's Knowledge of the Situations that Provoke Emotion , 1987 .

[41]  J. Landman Regret and Elation Following Action and Inaction , 1987 .

[42]  Susan Harter,et al.  Developmental changes in children's understanding of single, multiple, and blended emotion concepts. , 1989 .

[43]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives , 1986 .

[44]  Ross A. Thompson Development of children's inferences of the emotions of others. , 1987 .

[45]  B. Mellers,et al.  Anticipated Emotions as Guides to Choice , 2001 .

[46]  N. Sanders,et al.  Journal of behavioral decision making: "The need for contextual and technical knowledge in judgmental forecasting", 5 (1992) 39-52 , 1992 .

[47]  Kevin J. Riggs,et al.  Children's Reasoning and the Mind , 2000 .

[48]  Susanne A. Denham,et al.  Socialization of Preschoolers' Emotion Understanding. , 1994 .