Effects of proprioceptive feedback and environmental characteristics on spatial learning in virtual environments

The effect of proprioceptive information and environmental characteristics on spatial learning was investigated when participants repeatedly navigated complex three-dimensional virtual mazes. Proprioceptive information, provided by viewing the mazes using a head-mounted display, was found to have little effect. The primary environmental characteristics were layout orthogonality (using paths that intersected at either oblique or 90° angles), lines of sight (controlled using computer-generated "fog"), a visually defined perimeter and global landmarks. Participants travelled less far in orthogonal than oblique environments, even when fog was used to make the distance that participants could see equivalent. The removal of fog caused a further, substantial reduction in the distance participants travelled, indicating the importance of extended lines of sight, as predicted by the architectural theory of space syntax. Global landmarks promoted a similar rate of spatial learning to a visual perimeter.

[1]  G. Allen,et al.  The role of perceptual context in structuring spatial knowledge. , 1978 .

[2]  A Berthoz,et al.  Parietal and hippocampal contribution to topokinetic and topographic memory. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[3]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[4]  D. R. Montello A New Framework for Understanding the Acquisition of Spatial Knowledge in Large-Scale Environments , 1998 .

[5]  C. T. Validity,et al.  The Measurement of Cognitive Distance: Methods and Construct Validity , 2005 .

[6]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[7]  Rudy Darken,et al.  Navigating large virtual spaces , 1996, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[8]  David Banister,et al.  Configurational Modelling of Urban Movement Networks , 1998 .

[9]  T. Gärling,et al.  The effects of pathway configuration, landmarks and stress on environmental cognition , 1984 .

[10]  C C Presson,et al.  Updating after Rotational and Translational Body Movements: Coordinate Structure of Perspective Space , 1994, Perception.

[11]  John R. Wilson,et al.  Virtual Reality-Induced Symptoms and Effects (VRISE) , 1999, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[12]  Max J. Egenhofer,et al.  Spatial and temporal reasoning in geographic information systems , 1998 .

[13]  Roy A. Ruddle,et al.  Navigating Large-Scale Virtual Environments: What Differences Occur Between Helmet-Mounted and Desk-Top Displays? , 1999, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[14]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[15]  James P. Bliss,et al.  The Effectiveness of Virtual Reality for Administering Spatial Navigation Training to Firefighters , 1997, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[16]  Stuart C. Grant,et al.  Contributions of Proprioception to Navigation in Virtual Environments , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[17]  G W Evans,et al.  Cognitive mapping: knowledge of real-world distance and location information. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[18]  Reginald G. Golledge Cognition of physical and built environments , 1991 .

[19]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Navigating Buildings in "Desk-Top" Virtual Environments: Experimental Investigations Using Extended Navigational Experience , 1997 .

[20]  T. Gärling,et al.  Environment, cognition, and action : an integrated approach , 1991 .

[21]  N. Gale,et al.  Exploring the anchor-point hypothesis of spatial cognition , 1987 .

[22]  M. May,et al.  Navigating in a Virtual Environment With Map-Acquired Knowledge: Encoding and Alignment Effects , 1995 .

[23]  A RuddleRoy,et al.  Effects of proprioceptive feedback and environmental characteristics on spatial learning in virtual environments , 2004 .

[24]  Peter J. Werkhoven,et al.  The Effects of Proprioceptive and Visual Feedback on Geographical Orientation in Virtual Environments , 1999, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[25]  Karin Schweizer,et al.  Oblique Angled Intersections and Barriers: Navigating through a Virtual Maze , 2000, Spatial Cognition.

[26]  J. Rieser Access to Knowledge of Spatial Structure at Novel Points of Observation , 1989 .

[27]  Matt J. Rossano,et al.  Spatial representations acquired from computer models : Cognitive load, orientation specificity and the acquisition of survey knowledge , 1998 .

[28]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  The effects of maps on navigation and search strategies in very-large-scale virtual environments , 1999 .

[29]  Roy A. Ruddle,et al.  The effects of hyperlinks on navigation in virtual environments , 2000, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[30]  B. Tversky Distortions in memory for maps , 1981, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  J. Rieser Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[32]  A RuddleRoy,et al.  Navigating Large-Scale Virtual Environments , 1999 .

[33]  Richard B. Chase,et al.  THE ACQUISITION OF ROUTE AND SURVEY KNOWLEDGE FROM COMPUTER MODELS , 1999 .

[34]  E. Sadalla,et al.  Reference points in spatial cognition. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[35]  Hanspeter A. Mallot,et al.  The Role of Global and Local Landmarks in Virtual Environment Navigation , 2000, Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ..

[36]  Sarah S. Chance,et al.  Spatial Updating of Self-Position and Orientation During Real, Imagined, and Virtual Locomotion , 1998 .

[37]  E. Sadalla,et al.  Reference points in spatial cognition. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[38]  J. Peponis,et al.  Finding the Building in Wayfinding , 1990 .

[39]  Daniel R. Montello,et al.  The measurement of cognitive distance: Methods and construct validity , 1991 .

[40]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Locomotion Mode Affects the Updating of Objects Encountered During Travel: The Contribution of Vestibular and Proprioceptive Inputs to Path Integration , 1998, Presence.

[41]  P Péruch,et al.  Homing in Virtual Environments: Effects of Field of View and Path Layout , 1997, Perception.

[42]  Kevin Lynch,et al.  The Image of the City , 1960 .

[43]  Alan Penn,et al.  Natural Movement: Or, Configuration and Attraction in Urban Pedestrian Movement , 1993 .