From bird calls to human language: exploring the evolutionary drivers of compositional syntax
暂无分享,去创建一个
Toshitaka N Suzuki | David Wheatcroft | Michael Griesser | D. Wheatcroft | M. Griesser | Toshitaka N. Suzuki
[1] R. Seyfarth,et al. Vervet monkey alarm calls: Manipulation through shared information? , 1985 .
[2] Toshitaka N Suzuki,et al. Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls , 2016, Nature Communications.
[3] Gábor Győri. Animal communication and human language , 1995 .
[4] Nobuyuki Kutsukake,et al. Foraging intention affects whether willow tits call to attract members of mixed-species flocks , 2017, Royal Society Open Science.
[5] K. Riebel,et al. Are good ornaments bad armaments? Male chaffinch perception of songs with varying flourish length , 2003, Animal Behaviour.
[6] Simon W. Townsend,et al. Communication: Animal Steps on the Road to Syntax? , 2017, Current Biology.
[7] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Campbell's Monkeys Use Affixation to Alter Call Meaning , 2009, PloS one.
[8] J. Hoogland,et al. Nepotism and alarm calling in the black-tailed prairie dog (Cynomys ludovicianus) , 1983, Animal Behaviour.
[9] B. Hewlett,et al. Co-Residence Patterns in Hunter-Gatherer Societies Show Unique Human Social Structure , 2011, Science.
[10] Shinichi Nakagawa,et al. Family living sets the stage for cooperative breeding and ecological resilience in birds , 2017, PLoS biology.
[11] Michael Griesser,et al. Predation risk drives the expression of mobbing across bird species , 2017 .
[12] C. Scharff,et al. Twitter evolution: converging mechanisms in birdsong and human speech , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[13] M. Nowak,et al. Towards an evolutionary theory of language , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[14] Michael Griesser,et al. Referential Calls Signal Predator Behavior in a Group-Living Bird Species , 2008, Current Biology.
[15] Fernando Nottebohm,et al. Vocal learning in birds and humans. , 2003, Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews.
[16] Michael Griesser. Do warning calls boost survival of signal recipients? Evidence from a field experiment in a group-living bird species , 2013, Frontiers in Zoology.
[17] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Call combinations in monkeys: Compositional or idiomatic expressions? , 2012, Brain and Language.
[18] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Long-Distance Calling by the Willow Tit, Poecile montanus, Facilitates Formation of Mixed-Species Foraging Flocks , 2012 .
[19] P. Sherman. Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. , 1977, Science.
[20] Todd M. Freeberg,et al. Complexity in the Chick-a-Dee Call of Carolina Chickadees (Poecile Carolinensis): Associations of Context and Signaler Behavior to Call Structure , 2008 .
[21] Caroline Schuppli,et al. The Ecology of Social Learning in Animals and its Link with Intelligence , 2016, The Spanish Journal of Psychology.
[22] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Communication about predator type by a bird using discrete, graded and combinatorial variation in alarm calls , 2014, Animal Behaviour.
[23] Sabrina Engesser,et al. Meaningful call combinations and compositional processing in the southern pied babbler , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[24] Judith M Burkart,et al. Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution , 2009 .
[25] J. Ekman,et al. Nepotistic alarm calling in the Siberian jay, Perisoreus infaustus , 2004, Animal Behaviour.
[26] Sarah Wong,et al. The Mitochondrial Lon Protease Is Required for Age-Specific and Sex-Specific Adaptation to Oxidative Stress , 2017, Current Biology.
[27] David Wheatcroft,et al. Wild Birds Use anOrdering Rule to DecodeNovel Call Sequences Graphical Abstract Highlights , 2017 .
[28] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Campbell's monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[29] Bernard T. Tervoort,et al. You me downtown movie fun , 1968 .
[30] Michael Griesser,et al. The alarm call system of breeding Brown Thornbills (Acanthiza pusilla): self-defence or nest defence? , 2014, Journal of Ornithology.
[31] G. Roberts,et al. Cooperation through interdependence , 2005, Animal Behaviour.
[32] Robin I. M. Dunbar. Groups, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language , 1996 .
[33] M. Manser,et al. Segmental concatenation of individual signatures and context cues in banded mongoose (Mungos mungo) close calls , 2012, BMC Biology.
[34] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Semantic communication in birds: evidence from field research over the past two decades , 2016, Ecological Research.
[35] Michael Griesser,et al. Naive Juveniles Are More Likely to Become Breeders after Witnessing Predator Mobbing , 2017, The American Naturalist.
[36] Noam Chomsky,et al. How Could Language Have Evolved? , 2014, PLoS biology.
[37] van Schaik,et al. The Primate Origins of Human Nature , 2016 .
[38] P. Slater,et al. Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations , 1995 .
[39] S. E. Campbell,et al. Alternative forms of song matching in song sparrows , 2002, Animal Behaviour.
[40] Philippe Schlenker,et al. What Do Monkey Calls Mean? , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[41] R. Seyfarth,et al. The evolution of language from social cognition , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[42] J. Hare,et al. The first cut is the deepest: primary syllables of Richardson's ground squirrel, Spermophilus richardsonii, repeated calls alert receivers , 2008, Animal Behaviour.
[43] Peter Marler,et al. Bird Calls: Their Potential for Behavioral Neurobiology , 2004, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[44] C. Darwin. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex: INDEX , 1871 .
[45] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Calling at a Food Source: Context-Dependent Variation in Note Composition of Combinatorial Calls in Willow Tits , 2012 .
[46] Simon W. Townsend,et al. Experimental Evidence for Phonemic Contrasts in a Nonhuman Vocal System , 2015, PLoS biology.
[47] M. Griesser. Mobbing calls signal predator category in a kin group-living bird species , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.