The architectural colour design process: An evaluation of sequential media via semantic ratings

In recent studies, contextual situations of applied colours are compared to colours presented as samples or chips. Findings of such studies point out different results in terms of similarities or differences between the evaluations of isolated/abstract colours and contextualized situations. Architectural and spatial contexts have their own characteristics regarding colouring criteria, so it is of great importance to examine the architectural/spatial colouring process from this point of view. This study explores this process by investigating the consistency of semantic ratings of four sequential stages of the architectural colour design process, namely, colour chips/samples, abstract compositions, perspective drawings and 3D models. The architectural context for the study was a simple interior space. Fifteen different colour schemes were applied on the four media representing the stages. Subjects rated the 15 sets against seven bipolar, five-step semantic differential scales. The scales consisted of harmonious-discord, pleasant-unpleasant, comfortable-uncomfortable, spacious-confined, static-dynamic, exciting-calming and extroverted-introverted. Findings indicated that there are significant associations between the evaluations of the abstract compositions, the perspective drawings and the 3D models; however, the evaluations of colour chips are significantly different than the others. The medium effect observed mostly between abstract and contextualized media. Additionally, factor analysis showed that pleasantness, harmony, spaciousness and comfort are connected in the evaluations of contextual situations, while pleasantness and harmony differ from spaciousness and comfort in the evaluations of colour chips and abstract compositions. The factor of activity (arousal) (dynamism, excitement, and extroversion) stays the same for all four media. It is also found that different colour characteristics are determinative over different media. © 2010 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2010

[1]  L. Ou,et al.  A study of colour emotion and colour preference. Part II: Colour emotions for two‐colour combinations , 2004 .

[2]  T. Whitfield,et al.  Esthetic decision‐making: How do people select colours for real settings? , 2008 .

[3]  J Hogg,et al.  Dimensions and determinants of judgements of colour samples and a simulated interior space by architects and non-architects. , 1979, British journal of psychology.

[4]  Monica Billger,et al.  Colour combination effects in experimental rooms , 1999 .

[5]  Leyla Dokuzer Öztürk,et al.  An Example of Facade Colour Design of Mass Housing , 2002 .

[6]  Saadet Akbay AN ANALYSIS OF BASIC DESIGN STUDENTS' INTUITIVE AND ANALYTIC ATTITUDES IN COLOUR DECISIONS , 2003 .

[7]  Monica Billger,et al.  Significance of Colour on Room Character: Study on Dominantly Reddish and Greenish Colours in North- and South-facing Rooms , 2007 .

[8]  A. Mehrabian Pleasure-arousal-dominance: A general framework for describing and measuring individual differences in Temperament , 1996 .

[9]  B. Mikellides,et al.  Color research in architectural education—A cross‐cultural explorative study , 1998 .

[10]  Charles Taft,et al.  COLOR MEANING AND CONTEXT : COMPARISONS OF SEMANTIC RATINGS OF COLORS ON SAMPLES AND OBJECTS , 1997 .

[11]  L. Ou,et al.  A colour harmony model for two-colour combinations , 2006 .

[12]  Paul Green-Armytage,et al.  The value of knowledge for colour design , 2006 .

[13]  L. Sivik,et al.  A Theory of Colors in Combination-A Descriptive Model Related to the NCS Color-Order System , 2001 .

[14]  A. Nemcsics Experimental Determination of Laws of Color Harmony. Part 1: Harmony Content of Different Scales with Similar Hue , 2007 .

[15]  Dianne J. Smith Environmental colouration and/or the design process , 2003 .