Proprioception Is Robust under External Forces

Information from cutaneous, muscle and joint receptors is combined with efferent information to create a reliable percept of the configuration of our body (proprioception). We exposed the hand to several horizontal force fields to examine whether external forces influence this percept. In an end-point task subjects reached visually presented positions with their unseen hand. In a vector reproduction task, subjects had to judge a distance and direction visually and reproduce the corresponding vector by moving the unseen hand. We found systematic individual errors in the reproduction of the end-points and vectors, but these errors did not vary systematically with the force fields. This suggests that human proprioception accounts for external forces applied to the hand when sensing the position of the hand in the horizontal plane.

[1]  M. Landy,et al.  A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues , 1993, Vision Research.

[2]  Uwe Proske,et al.  Signals of motor command bias joint position sense in the presence of feedback from proprioceptors. , 2009, Journal of applied physiology.

[3]  Elizabeth T. Wilson,et al.  Mapping Proprioception across a 2D Horizontal Workspace , 2010, PloS one.

[4]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  E. Brenner,et al.  Illusions as a tool to study the coding of pointing movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Martha Flanders,et al.  Sensory control of target acquisition , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[7]  J.B.J. Smeets,et al.  A new binocular cue for absolute distance: Disparity relative to the most distant structure , 2010, Vision Research.

[8]  D. McCloskey Kinesthetic sensibility. , 1978, Physiological reviews.

[9]  Mark Mulder,et al.  Haptic shared control: smoothly shifting control authority? , 2012, Cognition, Technology & Work.

[10]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  D. Wolpert,et al.  When Feeling Is More Important Than Seeing in Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2002, Current Biology.

[12]  Anne C. Sittig,et al.  The precision of proprioceptive position sense , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Reweighting visual cues by touch. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[14]  Jeroen B. J. Smeets,et al.  Muscular Torque Can Explain Biases in Haptic Length Perception: A Model Study on the Radial-Tangential Illusion , 2010, EuroHaptics.

[15]  P. Matthews MUSCLE SPINDLES AND THEIR MOTOR CONTROL. , 1964, Physiological reviews.

[16]  Knut Drewing,et al.  Optimal integration of visual and proprioceptive movement information for the perception of trajectory geometry , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Michael S Landy,et al.  Motor learning reveals the existence of multiple codes for movement planning. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Dinant A. Kistemaker,et al.  In Vivo Dynamics of the Musculoskeletal System Cannot Be Adequately Described Using a Stiffness-Damping-Inertia Model , 2011, PloS one.

[19]  Robert J. van Beers,et al.  How humans combine simultaneous proprioceptive and visual position information , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  E. Brenner,et al.  Misjudging where you felt a light switch in a dark room , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Mark Mulder,et al.  Sharing Control With Haptics , 2012, Hum. Factors.

[22]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Motor commands contribute to human position sense , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Sensory integration does not lead to sensory calibration , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  Christopher A. Buneo,et al.  The Proprioceptive Map of the Arm Is Systematic and Stable, but Idiosyncratic , 2011, PloS one.

[26]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Processes controlling arm movements in monkeys. , 1978, Science.

[27]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[28]  Frans C. T. van der Helm,et al.  A Task-Specific Analysis of the Benefit of Haptic Shared Control During Telemanipulation , 2013, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.