Proprioception Is Robust under External Forces
暂无分享,去创建一个
Eli Brenner | Jeroen B. J. Smeets | Irene A. Kuling | I. A. Kuling | E. Brenner | J. Smeets | I. Kuling
[1] M. Landy,et al. A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues , 1993, Vision Research.
[2] Uwe Proske,et al. Signals of motor command bias joint position sense in the presence of feedback from proprioceptors. , 2009, Journal of applied physiology.
[3] Elizabeth T. Wilson,et al. Mapping Proprioception across a 2D Horizontal Workspace , 2010, PloS one.
[4] F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[5] E. Brenner,et al. Illusions as a tool to study the coding of pointing movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[6] Martha Flanders,et al. Sensory control of target acquisition , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.
[7] J.B.J. Smeets,et al. A new binocular cue for absolute distance: Disparity relative to the most distant structure , 2010, Vision Research.
[8] D. McCloskey. Kinesthetic sensibility. , 1978, Physiological reviews.
[9] Mark Mulder,et al. Haptic shared control: smoothly shifting control authority? , 2012, Cognition, Technology & Work.
[10] R. J. van Beers,et al. Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.
[11] D. Wolpert,et al. When Feeling Is More Important Than Seeing in Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2002, Current Biology.
[12] Anne C. Sittig,et al. The precision of proprioceptive position sense , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] Eli Brenner,et al. Reweighting visual cues by touch. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[14] Jeroen B. J. Smeets,et al. Muscular Torque Can Explain Biases in Haptic Length Perception: A Model Study on the Radial-Tangential Illusion , 2010, EuroHaptics.
[15] P. Matthews. MUSCLE SPINDLES AND THEIR MOTOR CONTROL. , 1964, Physiological reviews.
[16] Knut Drewing,et al. Optimal integration of visual and proprioceptive movement information for the perception of trajectory geometry , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] Michael S Landy,et al. Motor learning reveals the existence of multiple codes for movement planning. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.
[18] Dinant A. Kistemaker,et al. In Vivo Dynamics of the Musculoskeletal System Cannot Be Adequately Described Using a Stiffness-Damping-Inertia Model , 2011, PloS one.
[19] Robert J. van Beers,et al. How humans combine simultaneous proprioceptive and visual position information , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] E. Brenner,et al. Misjudging where you felt a light switch in a dark room , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[21] Mark Mulder,et al. Sharing Control With Haptics , 2012, Hum. Factors.
[22] S. Gandevia,et al. Motor commands contribute to human position sense , 2006, The Journal of physiology.
[23] Philip N. Sabes,et al. Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[24] Eli Brenner,et al. Sensory integration does not lead to sensory calibration , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[25] Christopher A. Buneo,et al. The Proprioceptive Map of the Arm Is Systematic and Stable, but Idiosyncratic , 2011, PloS one.
[26] E. Bizzi,et al. Processes controlling arm movements in monkeys. , 1978, Science.
[27] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[28] Frans C. T. van der Helm,et al. A Task-Specific Analysis of the Benefit of Haptic Shared Control During Telemanipulation , 2013, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.