Aligning mispronounced words to meaning: Evidence from ERP and reaction time studies

Many models have been proposed to account for the role that the mental lexicon plays in the initial stages of speech perception. One fundamental disparity between these models is how speech is phonologically represented in the mental lexicon. Theories range from full specification and representation of all phonological information to sparse specification. We report on two perception experiments using context independent mispronunciations (i.e. mispronunciations not governed by phonological rules) to test the predictions of the two most divergent models. Models assuming full specification and storage of all phonological information (e.g. exemplar models) predict symmetric acceptance or rejection of mispronunciations that only differ from real words in place of articulation of the medial consonant (*temor-tenor, *inage-image). Models assuming that only contrasting phonological information is stored (as in FUL) predict asymmetric patterns of acceptance, i.e. mispronunciations with medial coronal consonants will be better tolerated (*temor) than mispronunciations with medial labial or velar consonants. Results of two experiments using lexical decision with semantic priming in British English reveal an asymmetry in the acceptance of mispronunciations for coronal vs. noncoronal consonants. Both reaction time latencies as well as N400 amplitudes exhibit asymmetries, supporting the notion of abstract asymmetric lexical representation.

[1]  D. Gow Does English coronal place assimilation create lexical ambiguity , 2002 .

[2]  Claudia K. Friedrich,et al.  Neurophysiological evidence for underspecified lexical representations: asymmetries with word initial variations. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Aditi Lahiri,et al.  Gesture, Segment, Prosody: Lexical processing and phonological representation , 1992 .

[4]  Keith Johnson,et al.  Decisions and mechanisms in exemplar-based phonology , 2005 .

[5]  P. Holcomb,et al.  Cross-modal semantic priming: A time-course analysis using event-related brain potentials , 1993 .

[6]  Henning Reetz,et al.  Distinctive features: Phonological underspecification in representation and processing , 2010, J. Phonetics.

[7]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Phonological variation and inference in lexical access. , 1996 .

[9]  Cynthia M. Connine,et al.  Lexical representation of phonological variation in spoken word recognition , 2007 .

[10]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Phonological Variation in Lexical Access: Abstractness, Inference, and English Place Assimilation. , 1995 .

[11]  Dawn G. Blasko,et al.  Do the Beginnings of Spoken Words Have a Special Status in Auditory Word Recognition , 1993 .

[12]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.

[13]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .

[14]  Mark A Pitt,et al.  How are pronunciation variants of spoken words recognized? A test of generalization to newly learned words. , 2009, Journal of memory and language.

[15]  Mirjam Ernestus,et al.  Listeners recover /t/s that speakers reduce: Evidence from /t/-lenition in Dutch , 2006, J. Phonetics.

[16]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[17]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Mechanisms of phonological inference in speech perception. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Kara D. Federmeier,et al.  Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  Aditi Lahiri,et al.  Inequality across consonantal contrasts in speech perception: evidence from mismatch negativity. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  D. Gow Feature parsing: Feature cue mapping in spoken word recognition , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[22]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  The mental representation of lexical form: A phonological approach to the recognition lexicon , 1991, Cognition.

[23]  Claudia K. Friedrich,et al.  Neurophysiological correlates of mismatch in lexical access , 2005, BMC Neuroscience.

[24]  Linda Wheeldon,et al.  Phonological underspecification and mapping mechanisms in the speech recognition lexicon , 2004, Brain and Language.

[25]  P. Zwitserlood,et al.  Assimilation in existing and novel German compounds , 2005 .

[26]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Accessing Spoken Words: The Importance of Word Onsets , 1989 .

[27]  John Coleman,et al.  Stochastic phonological grammars and acceptability , 1997, SIGMORPHON@EACL.

[28]  Holger Mitterer,et al.  Coping with phonological assimilation in speech perception: Evidence for early compensation , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  Emanuel Donchin,et al.  The effect of handedness, the responding hand, and response force on the contralateral dominance of the readiness potential , 1977 .

[30]  Mathias Scharinger,et al.  ERP signatures of cross-modal semantic fragment priming: early context effects in speech perception. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[31]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Accessing Different Types of Lexical Semantic Information: Evidence From Priming , 1995 .

[32]  Jens Bölte,et al.  Is Phonological Information Mapped onto Semantic Information in a One-to-One Manner? , 2002, Brain and Language.

[33]  Keith Johnson,et al.  Vowel Discrimination by English, French and Turkish Speakers: Evidence for an Exemplar-Based Approach to Speech Perception , 2010, Phonetica.

[34]  Patrick Berg,et al.  Artifact Correction of the Ongoing EEG Using Spatial Filters Based on Artifact and Brain Signal Topographies , 2002, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[35]  Henning Reetz,et al.  When BEAT becomes HOUSE: Factors of word final /t/-deletion in German , 2011, Speech Commun..

[36]  A. Lahiri,et al.  “What you encode is not necessarily what you store”: Evidence for sparse feature representations from mismatch negativity , 2011, Brain Research.

[37]  R. Schvaneveldt,et al.  Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[38]  Natalie D. Snoeren,et al.  The impact of strong assimilation on the perception of connected speech. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  Walter Ritter,et al.  Repetition and semantic priming of nonwords: implications for theories of N400 and word recognition. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[40]  Aditi Lahiri,et al.  Not every pseudoword disrupts word recognition: an ERP study , 2006, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[41]  Claudia K. Friedrich,et al.  Neuronal spoken word recognition: The time course of processing variation in the speech signal , 2012 .