Touching words is not enough: How visual experience influences haptic–auditory associations in the “Bouba–Kiki” effect
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] C. Spence. Managing sensory expectations concerning products and brands: Capitalizing on the potential of sound and shape symbolism , 2012 .
[2] R. Davis. The fitness of names to drawings. A cross-cultural study in Tanganyika. , 1961, British journal of psychology.
[3] A. Vouloumanos,et al. Sound symbolism in infancy: evidence for sound-shape cross-modal correspondences in 4-month-olds. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[4] Amedeo D'Angiulli,et al. The effects of interference on recognition of haptic pictures in blindfolded sighted participants: the modality of representation of haptic information. , 2012, Scandinavian journal of psychology.
[5] Kurt E. Weaver,et al. Functional characteristics of auditory cortex in the blind , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.
[6] Charles Spence,et al. ‘Bouba’ and ‘Kiki’ in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape–sound matches, but different shape–taste matches to westerners , 2013 .
[7] Edward M. Hubbard,et al. Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia , 2005, Neuron.
[8] Charles Spence,et al. ‘The Sweet Taste of Maluma’: Crossmodal Associations Between Tastes and Words , 2012, Chemosensory Perception.
[9] C. Spence,et al. On the taste of “Bouba” and “Kiki”: An exploration of word–food associations in neurologically normal participants , 2011, Cognitive neuroscience.
[10] Vilayanur S Ramachandran,et al. Preliminary evidence for deficits in multisensory integration in autism spectrum disorders: The mirror neuron hypothesis , 2008, Social neuroscience.
[11] A. Paivio. Imagery and verbal processes , 1972 .
[12] E. Zohary,et al. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the occipital pole interferes with verbal processing in blind subjects , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[13] Amir Amedi,et al. Multisensory visual–tactile object related network in humans: insights gained using a novel crossmodal adaptation approach , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[14] P. Huttenlocher. Neural Plasticity: The Effects of Environment on the Development of the Cerebral Cortex , 2002 .
[15] R. Zatorre,et al. Crossmodal recruitment of primary visual cortex following brief exposure to bimodal audiovisual stimuli , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[16] R. Hobson,et al. The pathogenesis of autism: insights from congenital blindness. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[17] C. Spence,et al. “Bouba” and “Kiki” in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape–sound matches, but different shape–taste matches to Westerners , 2013, Cognition.
[18] Albert Postma,et al. Reference Frame Preferences in Haptics Differ for the Blind and Sighted in the Horizontal but Not in the Vertical Plane , 2011, Perception.
[19] Jill Whitehead,et al. What is audio description , 2005 .
[20] Amir Amedi,et al. A Putative Model of Multisensory Object Representation , 2009, Brain Topography.
[21] F. Rösler,et al. Altered auditory-tactile interactions in congenitally blind humans: an event-related potential study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[22] B. Röder,et al. Basic Multisensory Functions Can Be Acquired After Congenital Visual Pattern Deprivation in Humans , 2012, Developmental neuropsychology.
[23] M. Ryan,et al. What do animal signals mean? , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[24] F. Rösler,et al. Early visual deprivation impairs multisensory interactions in humans , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[25] D. Rendall,et al. The sound of round: evaluating the sound-symbolic role of consonants in the classic Takete-Maluma phenomenon. , 2011, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[26] M. Heller,et al. Superior Haptic Perceptual Selectivity in Late-Blind and Very-Low-Vision Subjects , 2003, Perception.
[27] V. Ramachandran,et al. Synaesthesia? A window into perception, thought and language , 2001 .
[28] A Koriat,et al. Figural symbolism in Chinese ideographs , 1979, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[29] C. Spence,et al. Spatial coordinate systems for tactile spatial attention depend on developmental vision: evidence from event‐related potentials in sighted and congenitally blind adult humans , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.
[30] David C. Reutens,et al. Early but not late-blindness leads to enhanced auditory perception , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[31] Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al. Behavioral and neuroplastic changes in the blind: evidence for functionally relevant cross-modal interactions , 2004, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[32] C. Spence,et al. Tasting shapes and words , 2011 .
[33] Asher Koriat,et al. The symbolic implications of vowels and of their orthographic representations in two natural languages , 1977 .
[34] Federico Fontana,et al. Association of Haptic Trajectories to Takete and Maluma , 2013, HAID.
[35] Stephan Gramley,et al. Cross-Cultural Pragmatics , 2008 .
[36] Yong Liu,et al. Altered Anatomical Network in Early Blindness Revealed by Diffusion Tensor Tractography , 2009, PloS one.
[37] D. Maurer,et al. The shape of boubas: sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. , 2006, Developmental science.
[38] Network 1000: Surveying the changing needs and circumstances of visually impaired adults in Great Britain , 2005 .
[39] T. Vecchi,et al. Blind Vision: The Neuroscience of Visual Impairment , 2011 .
[40] Ophelia Deroy,et al. Tasting Liquid Shapes: Investigating the Sensory Basis of Cross-modal Correspondences , 2011 .
[41] Charles Spence,et al. Assessing the shapes and speech sounds that people associate with chocolate samples varying in cocoa content , 2011 .
[42] M. Lassonde,et al. Cross-modal plasticity for the spatial processing of sounds in visually deprived subjects , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[43] S. Palazzi. Autism and Blindness. Research and Reflections , 2005 .