Evidence for use of the Quick Interactive Language Screener (QUILS™) to measure the relationship between socioeconomic status and language development1
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] G. Baird,et al. Socioeconomic disadvantage is associated with prevalence of developmental language disorders, but not rate of language or literacy growth in children from 4 to 11 years: evidence from the Surrey Communication and Language in Education Study (SCALES) , 2021, Social Science Research Network.
[2] L. Hand,et al. The experiences of whānau and kaiako with Speech-Language Therapy in Kaupapa Māori education , 2020 .
[3] Charles A Nelson,et al. Effects of poverty on interacting biological systems underlying child development. , 2017, The Lancet. Child & adolescent health.
[4] J. Adair,et al. How the Word Gap Argument Negatively Impacts Young Children of Latinx Immigrants' Conceptualizations of Learning. , 2017 .
[5] Eric J. Johnson,et al. Exposing Gaps in/Between Discourses of Linguistic Deficits , 2017, Critical Perspectives of the Language Gap.
[6] Marc W. Hernandez,et al. A parent-directed language intervention for children of low socioeconomic status: a randomized controlled pilot study* , 2015, Journal of Child Language.
[7] S. Pitama,et al. Improving Maori health through clinical assessment: Waikare o te Waka o Meihana. , 2014, The New Zealand medical journal.
[8] J. Lakin. Test Directions as a Critical Component of Test Design: Best Practices and the Impact of Examinee Characteristics , 2014 .
[9] David K. Dickinson,et al. Relation between language experiences in preschool classrooms and children's kindergarten and fourth-grade language and reading abilities. , 2011, Child development.
[10] J. Beitchman,et al. Twenty-year follow-up of children with and without speech-language impairments: family, educational, occupational, and quality of life outcomes. , 2010, American journal of speech-language pathology.
[11] M. Rowe,et al. Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[12] M. Pillay. Cross-Cultural Practice: What Is It Really About? , 2003, Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica.
[13] Linda Smith,et al. Decolonizing Methodologies: Research and Indigenous Peoples , 2000 .
[14] Robert Plomin,et al. Genetic influence on language delay in two-year-old children , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[15] Angela Losardo. Language Intervention , 1996 .
[16] Linda Tuhiwai Smith. Decolonizing Methodologies , 2021 .
[17] Diane M. Horm,et al. Language intervention research in early childhood care and education: A systematic survey of the literature , 2020 .
[18] Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al. Evaluating socioeconomic gaps in preschoolers’ vocabulary, syntax and language process skills with the Quick Interactive Language Screener (QUILS) , 2020 .
[19] E. Hoff. Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: implications for closing achievement gaps. , 2013, Developmental psychology.
[20] B. Dodd,et al. Childhood Bilingualism: Distinguishing Difference from Disorder , 2005 .