Acute cardiovascular exercise and executive control function.

Acute cardiovascular exercise effects on cognitive function were examined using an executive control task by comparing neuroelectric and behavioral performance at baseline with post-exercise in 20 undergraduates. A within-subjects design was used to assess the P3 component of an event-related brain potential (ERP) and behavioral performance using a task that varied the amount of executive control required. The baseline session involved participation on the Eriksen flankers task followed by a graded maximal exercise test to measure cardiovascular fitness. The exercise session consisted of a 30-min acute bout of exercise on a treadmill followed by the Eriksen flankers task after heart rate returned to within 10% of pre-exercise levels. Across midline recordings sites, results indicated larger P3 amplitude following acute exercise compared to baseline. Shorter P3 latency was observed during the baseline Eriksen flankers task for the neutral compared to the incompatible condition; an effect not found following the acute bout of exercise. These findings suggest that acute bouts of cardiovascular exercise affect neuroelectric processes underlying executive control through the increased allocation of neuroelectric resources and through changes in cognitive processing and stimulus classification speed.

[1]  T. Bashore,et al.  Age, physical fitness, and mental processing speed. , 1989, Annual review of gerontology & geriatrics.

[2]  A. Kramer,et al.  Perceptual organization and focused attention: The role of objects and proximity in visual processing , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  K M Spencer,et al.  The lateralized readiness potential: relationship between human data and response activation in a connectionist model. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[4]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[5]  Charles H Hillman,et al.  The relationship of age and cardiovascular fitness to cognitive and motor processes. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[6]  G. Borg Perceived exertion as an indicator of somatic stress. , 2019, Scandinavian journal of rehabilitation medicine.

[7]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .

[8]  E. Donchin,et al.  Performance of concurrent tasks: a psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources. , 1983, Science.

[9]  M Takahashi,et al.  The effect of jogging on P300 event related potentials. , 1999, Electromyography and clinical neurophysiology.

[10]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[11]  C W Eriksen,et al.  Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  J. Polich,et al.  Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review , 1995, Biological Psychology.

[13]  ROBERT W. Johnson,et al.  Healthy people. , 1985, Canadian journal of public health = Revue canadienne de sante publique.

[14]  P. Thompson,et al.  ACSM's Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription , 1995 .

[15]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[16]  C. C. Duncan-Johnson Young Psychophysiologist Award address, 1980. P300 latency: a new metric of information processing. , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[17]  W. Greenough,et al.  Exercise and the Brain: Angiogenesis in the Adult Rat Cerebellum after Vigorous Physical Activity and Motor Skill Learning , 1992, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[18]  R. Emmerson,et al.  Electrophysiology and Aging: Slowing, Inhibition, and Aerobic Fitness , 1990 .

[19]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Aging and inhibition: beyond a unitary view of inhibitory processing in attention. , 1994, Psychology and aging.

[20]  W. Spirduso,et al.  Physical fitness, aging, and psychomotor speed: a review. , 1980, Journal of gerontology.

[21]  Michael D. Jensen,et al.  Dose-response issues concerning physical activity and health: an evidence-based symposium. , 2001, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[22]  A. Goldberg,et al.  Impaired insulin sensitivity and maximal responsiveness in older hypertensive men. , 1994, Hypertension.

[23]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .

[24]  W. Chodzko-Zajko Physical fitness, cognitive performance, and aging. , 1991, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[25]  K. Kubitz,et al.  Does Aerobic Exercise Decrease Brain Activation , 1997 .

[26]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[27]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .

[28]  John Polich,et al.  EEG changes from long-term physical exercise , 1996, Biological Psychology.

[29]  K. Kubitz,et al.  EEG power spectral densities during and after cycle ergometer exercise. , 1996, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[30]  J. Polich,et al.  On the relationship between EEG and P300: individual differences, aging, and ultradian rhythms. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[31]  C. Dolisi,et al.  P300, N400, aerobic fitness, and maximal aerobic exercise. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[32]  Catherine R. Harrison,et al.  Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function , 1999, Nature.

[33]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[34]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 and long-term physical exercise. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[35]  M. Stones,et al.  Cognitive and Behavioral Performance Factors in Atypical Aging , 1990, Springer New York.