Abstraction as a natural process of mental compression

This paper considers mathematical abstraction as arising through a natural mechanism of the biological brain in which complicated phenomena are compressed into thinkable concepts. The neurons in the brain continually fire in parallel and the brain copes with the saturation of information by the simple expedient of suppressing irrelevant data and focusing only on a few important aspects at any given time. Language enables important phenomena to be named as thinkable concepts that can then be refined in meaning and connected together into coherent frameworks. Gray and Tall (1994) noted how this happened with the symbols of arithmetic, yielding a spectrum of performance between the more successful who used the symbols as thinkable concepts operating dually as process and concept (procept) and those who focused more on the step-by-step procedures and could perform simple arithmetic but failed to cope with more sophisticated problems. In this paper, we broaden the discussion to the full range of mathematics from the young child to the mature mathematician, and we support our analysis by reviewing a range of recent research studies carried out internationally by research students at the University of Warwick.

[1]  J. Piaget,et al.  The principles of genetic epistemology , 1972 .

[2]  Mercedes A. McGowen Cognitive Units, Concept Images, and Cognitive Collages: An Examination of the Processes of Knowledge Construction , 1998 .

[3]  J. Bruner Toward a Theory of Instruction , 1966 .

[4]  E. Jablonka International group for the psychology of mathematics education , 2008 .

[5]  Thomas J. Cooper,et al.  Fractions, reunitisation and the number-line representation , 1999 .

[6]  William Kessen,et al.  A Theory of Instruction , 1966 .

[7]  E. Rosch,et al.  Cognition and Categorization , 1980 .

[8]  D. Tall Differing Modes of Proof and Belief in Mathematics , 2002 .

[9]  Mike Askew,et al.  PUPILS' PARTICIPATION IN THE CLASSROOM EXAMINED IN RELATION TO 'INTERACTIVE WHOLE CLASS TEACHING' , 2001 .

[10]  David Tall,et al.  THINKING THROUGH THREE WORLDS OF MATHEMATICS , 2004 .

[11]  David Tall,et al.  Symbols and the Bifurcation Between Procedural and Conceptual Thinking , 2001 .

[12]  David Tall,et al.  Advanced Mathematical Thinking , 1994 .

[13]  R. Skemp The psychology of learning mathematics , 1979 .

[14]  G. Edelman,et al.  Consciousness : how matter becomes imagination , 2001 .

[15]  G. Hanna,et al.  Explanation and proof in mathematics : philosophical and educational perspectives , 2009 .

[16]  Ed Dubinsky,et al.  Reflective Abstraction in Advanced Mathematical Thinking , 2002 .

[17]  Vadim Andreevich Krutet︠s︡kiĭ The Psychology of Mathematical Abilities in Schoolchildren , 1976 .

[18]  Maselan Bin Ali Symbolic manipulations related to certain aspects such as interpretations of graphs , 1996 .

[19]  William P. Thurston,et al.  Mathematical Education , 2005, math/0503081.

[20]  P. Cobb,et al.  A Constructivist Alternative to the Representational View of Mind in Mathematics Education. , 1992 .

[21]  F. Attneave,et al.  The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory , 1949 .

[22]  A. Sfard On the dual nature of mathematical conceptions: Reflections on processes and objects as different sides of the same coin , 1991 .

[23]  D. Tall The Cognitive Development of Proof: Is Mathematical Proof For All or For Some? , 1998 .

[24]  David Tall,et al.  Knowledge construction and diverging thinking in elementary & advanced mathematics , 1999 .

[25]  Maria Doritou Understanding the number line: conception and practice , 2006 .

[26]  Kevin F. Collis,et al.  Evaluating the Quality of Learning: The SOLO Taxonomy , 1977 .

[27]  David Tall,et al.  The fundamental cycle of concept construction underlying various theoretical frameworks , 2005 .

[28]  Juan Pablo Mejía-Ramos,et al.  THE LONG-TERM COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT OF DIFFERENT TYPES OF REASONING AND PROOF , 2007 .

[29]  F. Crick The Astonishing Hypothesis , 1994 .

[30]  David Tall,et al.  Ambiguity and flexibility: A proceptual view of simple arithmetic , 1983 .

[31]  Eleanor Rosch,et al.  Principles of Categorization , 1978 .

[32]  Kate Wall,et al.  Interactive whole class teaching , 2003 .

[33]  D. Tirosh,et al.  Simplifying Algebraic Expressions: Teacher Awareness and Teaching Approaches , 1998 .

[34]  K. Maccorquodale Organization of Behavior : A Neuropsychological Theory , 1951 .

[35]  M. Mitchelmore,et al.  Conceptual Knowledge in Introductory Calculus. , 1996 .

[36]  J. Pegg Fundamental Cycles of Cognitive Growth. , 2002 .

[37]  Mike Thomas,et al.  What is the object of the encapsulation of a process , 1999 .

[38]  David Tall,et al.  Relationships between embodied objects and symbolic procepts: an explanatory theory of success and failure in mathematics , 2004 .

[39]  R. Skemp,et al.  Intelligence, Learning and Action , 1980 .

[40]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Bright Air, Brilliant Fire: On the Matter of the Mind , 1992 .