Bridging Token Identity Theory and Supervenience Theory Through Psychological Construction

A psychologist’s task is to discover facts about the mind by measuring responses at the level of a person (e.g., reaction times, perceptions, eye or muscle movements, or bodily changes). A neuroscientist’s task is to make similar discoveries by measuring responses from neurons in a brain (e.g., electrical, magnetic, blood flow or chemical measures related neurons firing). Both psychologists and neuroscientists use ideas (in the form of concepts, categories, and constructs) to transform their measurements into something meaningful. The relation between any set of numbers (reflecting a property of the person, or the activation in a set of neurons, a circuit, or a network) and a psychological construct is a psychometric issue that is formalized as a “measurement model.” The relation is also a philosophical act. Scientists (both neuroscientists and psychologists) who make such inferences, but don’t explicitly declare their measurement models, are still doing philosophy, but they are doing it in stealth, enacting certain assumptions that are left unsaid. In “Mind the Gap,” Kievit and colleagues (this issue) take the admirable step of trying to unmask the measurement models that lurk within two well-defined traditions for linking the actions of neurons to the actions of people. They translate identity theory and supervenience theory into popular measurement models that exist in psychometric theory using the logic and language of structural equation modeling. By showing that both philosophical approaches can be represented as models that relate measurements to psychological constructs, Kievit et al. lay bare the fact that all measurement questions are also philosophical questions about how variation in numbers hint at or point to reality. They make the powerful point that translating philosophical assumptions into psychometric terms allows both identity theory and supervenience theory to be treated like hypotheses that can be empirically evaluated and compared in more or less a concrete way. The empirical example offered by Kievit et al. (linking intelligence to brain volume) is somewhat simplistic on both the neuroscience and psychological ends of the equation, and the nitty-gritty details of applying an explicit measurement approach to more complex data remains open, but this article represents a big step forward in negotiating the chasm between measures taken at the level of the brain and those taken at the level of the person. The overall approach is applauded, but a closer look at the details of how Kievit et al. operationalized identity and supervenience theory is in order. In science, as in philosophy, the devil is in the details. In the pages that follow, I highlight a few lurking demons that haunt the Kievit et al. approach. I don’t point out every idea that I take issue with in the article, just as I don’t congratulate every point of agreement. Instead, I focus in on a few key issues in formalizing identity and supervenience theory, with an eye to asking whether they are really all that different in measurement terms, as well as whether standard psychometric models can be used to operationalize each of them equally well. Like Kievit et al., I conclude that a supervenience theory might win the day, but I try to get more specific about a version of supervenience that would successfully bridges the gap between the brain and the mind.

[1]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.

[2]  H. Reichenbach Experience And Prediction , 1938 .

[3]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[4]  P. Ekman,et al.  What is Meant by Calling Emotions Basic , 2011 .

[5]  R. Levenson Basic Emotion Questions , 2011 .

[6]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience: Understanding the Mind by Simulating the Brain , 2000 .

[7]  J. Coan Emergent Ghosts of the Emotion Machine , 2010 .

[8]  Metaphysics of consciousness , 1991 .

[9]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  On the automaticity of emotion , 2007 .

[10]  L. F. Barrett Variety is the spice of life: A psychological construction approach to understanding variability in emotion , 2009, Cognition & emotion.

[11]  R. Commissaris,et al.  Effects of lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala on anxiety-like behaviors in the rat , 1992, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[12]  D. Chalmers The Conscious Mind , 1996 .

[13]  A. Calder,et al.  Disgust discussed , 2003, Annals of neurology.

[14]  Jerome A. Shaffer,et al.  Mental events and the brain , 1963 .

[15]  Kristen A. Lindquist,et al.  The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review , 2012, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[16]  M. Gendron,et al.  Reconstructing the Past: A Century of Ideas About Emotion in Psychology , 2009, Emotion review : journal of the International Society for Research on Emotion.

[17]  Stephen José Hanson,et al.  Decoding the Large-Scale Structure of Brain Function by Classifying Mental States Across Individuals , 2009, Psychological science.

[18]  Jaegwon Kim,et al.  Making Sense of Emergence , 1999 .

[19]  Moshe Bar,et al.  See it with feeling: affective predictions during object perception , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  Randy L. Buckner,et al.  Unrest at rest: Default activity and spontaneous network correlations , 2007, NeuroImage.

[21]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  Affect as a Psychological Primitive. , 2009, Advances in experimental social psychology.

[22]  De Vries Book review: R.C. O'Reilly and Y. Munakata: Computational explorations in cognitive neuroscience: understanding the mind by stimulating the brain. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press. , 2002 .

[23]  R. Lennox,et al.  Conventional wisdom on measurement: A structural equation perspective. , 1991 .

[24]  J. Russell Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. , 2003, Psychological review.

[25]  Michael Davis,et al.  The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. , 1992, Annual review of neuroscience.

[26]  Michael L. Anderson Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[27]  Andrew M. Poulos,et al.  The neuroscience of mammalian associative learning. , 2005, Annual review of psychology.

[28]  N. Henry Problems of Life and Mind , 2000 .

[29]  John R. Searle,et al.  The Rediscovery of the Mind , 1995, Artif. Intell..

[30]  C. Izard Forms and Functions of Emotions: Matters of Emotion–Cognition Interactions , 2011 .

[31]  Lisa Feldman Barrett,et al.  Functional grouping and cortical–subcortical interactions in emotion: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies , 2008, NeuroImage.

[32]  Andrew Ortony,et al.  Appraisal theories: How cognition shapes affect into emotion. , 2008 .

[33]  Kristen A. Lindquist,et al.  The Embodiment of Emotion , 2008 .

[34]  S. Paradiso The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life , 1998 .

[35]  J. Fodor Special sciences (or: The disunity of science as a working hypothesis) , 1974, Synthese.

[36]  P. Ekman An argument for basic emotions , 1992 .

[37]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[38]  Michael Davis,et al.  The amygdala , 2000, Current Biology.

[39]  D. Campbell ‘Downward Causation’ in Hierarchically Organised Biological Systems , 1974 .

[40]  T. Dalgleish Basic Emotions , 2004 .

[41]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Conscious thought is for facilitating social and cultural interactions: how mental simulations serve the animal-culture interface. , 2010, Psychological review.

[42]  D. Armstrong,et al.  A Materialist Theory of the Mind , 2022 .

[43]  Lisa Feldman Barrett,et al.  Grounding emotion in situated conceptualization , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  M. Mesulam,et al.  From sensation to cognition. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  B. Biswal,et al.  Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo‐planar mri , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[46]  U. T. Place,et al.  Is consciousness a brain process? , 1956, British journal of psychology.

[47]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[48]  J. J. C. Smart,et al.  Sensations and brain processes , 1959 .

[49]  K. Maccorquodale,et al.  On a distinction between hypothetical constructs and intervening variables. , 1948, Psychological review.

[50]  S. Paradiso,et al.  Book Review: Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions , 2000 .

[51]  B. Mesquita,et al.  The experience of emotion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[52]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[53]  Lisa Feldman Barrett,et al.  Considering PTSD from the perspective of brain processes: a psychological construction approach. , 2011, Journal of traumatic stress.

[54]  J. Fuster The cognit: a network model of cortical representation. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[55]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  Handbook of emotions, 2nd ed. , 2000 .

[56]  L. F. Barrett Are Emotions Natural Kinds? , 2006, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[57]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[58]  John Dewey,et al.  The theory of emotion. , 1895 .

[59]  Kristen A. Lindquist,et al.  Constructing Emotion , 2008, Psychological science.

[60]  V. Menon,et al.  A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[61]  H. Putnam Mind, language, and reality , 1975 .

[62]  Mind—body identity, a side issue? , 1967 .

[63]  John A. Bargh,et al.  Social Psychology and the Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes , 2013 .

[64]  L. F. Barrett Solving the Emotion Paradox: Categorization and the Experience of Emotion , 2006, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[65]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[66]  Paul K. Feyerabend,et al.  Comment: ‘Mental events and the brain’ , 1963 .

[67]  Kristen A. Lindquist,et al.  Of Mice and Men: Natural Kinds of Emotions in the Mammalian Brain? A Response to Panksepp and Izard , 2007, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[68]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition , 2010, NeuroImage.

[69]  H. Reichenbach Experience and Prediction. An Analysis of the Foundations and the Structure of Knowledge , 1938 .

[70]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[71]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  Handbook of cognition and emotion , 1999 .

[72]  J. Bergenius Commentaries , 1996, Brain Research Bulletin.

[73]  G. Edelman Neural Darwinism: The Theory Of Neuronal Group Selection , 1989 .

[74]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[75]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  Handbook of Emotions , 1993 .

[76]  James L. McClelland Emergence in Cognitive Science , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[77]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Basolateral amygdala is not critical for cognitive memory of contextual fear conditioning. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[78]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  A default mode of brain function: A brief history of an evolving idea , 2007, NeuroImage.

[79]  J. Russell,et al.  The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology , 2005, Development and Psychopathology.

[80]  M. Fendt,et al.  The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of conditioned fear , 1999, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[81]  D. Dennett The Intentional Stance. , 1987 .

[82]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  Affect is a form of cognition: A neurobiological analysis , 2007, Cognition & emotion.

[83]  Lisa Feldman Barrett,et al.  The Future of Psychology Connecting Mind to Brain , 2009 .

[84]  Mark G. Baxter,et al.  Functional localization within the prefrontal cortex: missing the forest for the trees? , 2010, Trends in Neurosciences.

[85]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociable Intrinsic Connectivity Networks for Salience Processing and Executive Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[86]  Donald Davidson The Material Mind , 1973 .

[87]  Kristen A. Lindquist,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.8 Cognitive-emotional interactions Language as context for the , 2022 .