Age-related effects on lexical, but not syntactic, processes during sentence production
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A.P.A. Roelofs,et al. The architecture of speaking , 2019 .
[2] Susan Kemper,et al. Age differences in sentence production. , 2003, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.
[3] S. Sala,et al. The cumulative semantic interference effect in normal and pathological ageing , 2014, Neuropsychologia.
[4] Shravan Vasishth,et al. What did we learn from forty years of research on semantic interference? A Bayesian meta-analysis , 2020 .
[5] André Vandierendonck,et al. A comparison of methods to combine speed and accuracy measures of performance: A rejoinder on the binning procedure , 2016, Behavior Research Methods.
[6] P. Lachenbruch. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .
[7] D. Spieler,et al. Lexical competition and phonological encoding in young and older speakers. , 2006, Psychology and aging.
[8] Susan Kemper,et al. Effects of age, animacy and activation order on sentence production , 2006 .
[9] Zenzi M. Griffin,et al. The influence of age on the time course of word preparation in multiword utterances , 2006 .
[10] Deborah M. Burke,et al. Asymmetric aging effects on semantic and phonological processes: naming in the picture-word interference task. , 2002 .
[11] Franklin P. Tamborello,et al. Planning in sentence production: Evidence for the phrase as a default planning scope , 2010, Cognition.
[12] Mark C. Smith,et al. High level processing scope in spoken sentence production , 1999, Cognition.
[13] Sally Andrews,et al. To transform or not to transform: using generalized linear mixed models to analyse reaction time data , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[14] T. Jaeger,et al. Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.
[15] David A Balota,et al. Additive effects of word frequency and stimulus quality: the influence of trial history and data transformations. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[16] Elizabeth A. L. Stine-Morrow,et al. Self-regulated reading in adulthood. , 2008, Psychology and aging.
[17] Pascal Perrier,et al. Producing spoken sentences: The scope of incremental planning , 2012 .
[18] Susan Kemper,et al. Sentence production by young and older adults in controlled contexts. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.
[19] J. Villaseñor,et al. An R package for testing goodness of fit: goft , 2018 .
[20] James T. Townsend,et al. Methods of Modeling Capacity in Simple Processing Systems , 2014 .
[21] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Lexical Selection is Not a Competitive Process: A Reply to La Heij et al. (2006) , 2006, Cortex.
[22] L. Wheeldon,et al. Higher physical fitness levels are associated with less language decline in healthy ageing , 2018, Scientific Reports.
[23] Maverick E Smith,et al. Differential effects of knowledge and aging on the encoding and retrieval of everyday activities , 2019, Cognition.
[24] J. Jescheniak,et al. On the flexibility of grammatical advance planning during sentence production: Effects of cognitive load on multiple lexical access. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[25] Zhe Wang,et al. Culture Shapes Efficiency of Facial Age Judgments , 2010, PloS one.
[26] Kathryn Bock,et al. Language production : Grammatical encoding , 1994 .
[27] Arthur Wingfield,et al. Distraction by competing speech in young and older adult listeners. , 2002, Psychology and aging.
[28] Fernanda Ferreira,et al. Age Preservation of the Syntactic Processor in Production , 2003, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[29] Mark C. Smith,et al. Horizontal information flow in spoken sentence production. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[30] Jennifer C. Weeks,et al. The disruptive – and beneficial – effects of distraction on older adults’ cognitive performance , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[31] Colin Phillips,et al. The timing of verb selection in Japanese sentence production. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[32] S. Jay Samuels,et al. Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading , 1974 .
[33] Jeremy J Tree,et al. Sometimes faster, sometimes slower: associative and competitor priming in picture naming with young and elderly participants , 2003, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[34] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. Lexical search and order of mention in sentence production , 1981 .
[35] A. Wingfield,et al. Language and the aging brain: patterns of neural compensation revealed by functional brain imaging. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[36] Antje S. Meyer,et al. Incremental language production , 2003 .
[37] D. A. Kenny,et al. Experiments with More Than One Random Factor: Designs, Analytic Models, and Statistical Power , 2017, Annual review of psychology.
[38] L. Wheeldon,et al. Healthy Aging and Sentence Production: Disrupted Lexical Access in the Context of Intact Syntactic Planning , 2018, bioRxiv.
[39] André Vandierendonck,et al. Further Tests of the Utility of Integrated Speed-Accuracy Measures in Task Switching , 2018, Journal of cognition.
[40] D. Barr,et al. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.
[41] Randi C. Martin,et al. Working memory and planning during sentence production. , 2014, Acta psychologica.
[42] Ludovic Ferrand,et al. Automaticity of Word Reading , 2014 .
[43] S. Jay Samuels,et al. THE IMPORTANCE OF AUTOMATICITY FOR DEVELOPING EXPERTISE IN READING , 1997 .
[44] P. Starreveld,et al. Time-course analysis of semantic and orthographic context effects in picture naming. , 1996 .
[45] S. Cheimariou,et al. Semantic interference in a randomized naming task: Effects of age, order, and category , 2014, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[46] W. Levelt,et al. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .
[47] D. Mirman,et al. Distinct Effects of Lexical and Semantic Competition during Picture Naming in Younger Adults, Older Adults, and People with Aphasia , 2016, Front. Psychol..
[48] Pierre Bellec,et al. Task-Induced Functional Connectivity of Picture Naming in Healthy Aging: The Impacts of Age and Task Complexity , 2020, Neurobiology of Language.
[49] Jin-Chen Yang,et al. Horizontal Flow of Semantic and Phonological Information in Chinese Spoken Sentence Production , 2008, Language and speech.
[50] Donald G. MacKay,et al. On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? , 1991 .
[51] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. Lexical selection is not by competition: a reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[52] L. Wheeldon,et al. Healthy Aging and Sentence Production: Disrupted Lexical Access in the Context of Intact Syntactic Planning , 2020, Frontiers in Psychology.
[53] Davide Poggiali,et al. Raincloud plots: a multi-platform tool for robust data visualization , 2019, Wellcome open research.
[54] Nazar Zaki,et al. Applying the Inverse Efficiency Score to Visual–Motor Task for Studying Speed-Accuracy Performance While Aging , 2020, Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience.
[55] Renée DeCaro,et al. Prompting retrieval during monitoring and self-regulated learning in older and younger adults , 2020, Metacognition and Learning.
[56] A. Meyer,et al. Single and multiple object naming in healthy ageing , 2007 .
[57] D. Balota,et al. Mind-wandering in younger and older adults: converging evidence from the Sustained Attention to Response Task and reading for comprehension. , 2012, Psychology and aging.
[58] A. Meyer. Lexical Access in Phrase and Sentence Production: Results from Picture–Word Interference Experiments , 1996 .
[59] D. Burke. Language Production and Aging , 2002 .
[60] Linda Wheeldon,et al. Lexical availability and grammatical encoding scope during spoken sentence production , 2013, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[61] W. Glaser,et al. The time course of picture-word interference. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[62] F. Ferreira. Effects of length and syntactic complexity on initiation times for prepared utterances , 1991 .
[63] Antje S. Meyer,et al. Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .
[64] Leif D. Nelson,et al. False-Positive Psychology , 2011, Psychological science.
[65] Cristina D. Rabaglia,et al. Natural and constrained language production as a function of age and cognitive abilities , 2011, Language and cognitive processes.
[66] Anne Cutler,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[67] P. Verhaeghen,et al. Aging and verbal memory span: a meta-analysis. , 2005, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.
[68] C. Holmes,et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment: Validity and Utility in a Memory Clinic Setting , 2007, Canadian journal of psychiatry. Revue canadienne de psychiatrie.
[69] M. Vitevitch. Naturalistic and Experimental Analyses of Word Frequency and Neighborhood Density Effects in Slips of the Ear , 2002, Language and speech.
[70] T. Mitzner,et al. Language decline across the life span: findings from the Nun Study. , 2001, Psychology and aging.
[71] Arthur G Samuel,et al. A reason to rhyme: phonological and semantic influences on lexical access. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[72] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Power Analysis and Effect Size in Mixed Effects Models: A Tutorial , 2018, Journal of cognition.
[73] David J. Frank,et al. Validating older adults' reports of less mind-wandering: An examination of eye movements and dispositional influences. , 2015, Psychology and aging.
[74] D. Bates,et al. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.
[75] P. Verhaeghen. Aging and vocabulary scores: a meta-analysis. , 2003, Psychology and aging.
[76] S. Heim,et al. Why the leash constrains the dog: the impact of semantic associations on sentence production. , 2010, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.
[77] R. Ratcliff. Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.
[78] G S Dell,et al. A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.
[79] A. Roelofs,et al. The WEAVER model of word-form encoding in speech production , 1997, Cognition.
[80] Jacob Cohen. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.
[81] J. Cummings,et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2005, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.
[82] F. Ferreira,et al. How incremental is language production? Evidence from the production of utterances requiring the computation of arithmetic sums , 2002 .
[83] A. Roelofs,et al. A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking , 1992, Cognition.
[84] O. B. Emery. Language and aging. , 1985, Experimental aging research.
[85] S. Tipper. Less attentional selectivity as a result of declining inhibition in older adults , 1991 .
[86] E. Maylor,et al. Aging and Syntactic Representations: Evidence of Preserved Syntactic Priming and Lexical Boost , 2017, Psychology and aging.
[87] L. Wheeldon,et al. Planning scope in spoken sentence production: the role of grammatical units. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.