Activity patterns assessed throughout 24-hour recordings in preterm and near term infants.

The motility of 10 low-risk infants, aged between 34 and 40 weeks of postmenstrual age, has been continuously recorded for 24 h. Four codes were distinguished: code 1 (absence of motility or occasional occurrence of startles), code 2 (presence of small general or isolated body movements, startles, smiles, grimaces, and other facial activity), code 3 (forceful and prolonged general movements, startles, and stretches), code 4 (vigorous and abrupt general body movements accompanied by crying). Changes with age concern mainly the increase of the duration of code 1 (quiescence) episodes. Confrontation between day and night showed higher levels of motility during the night than during the day. The last weeks before term represent a time for increase in the ability to sustain a quiet behavior and to reorganize day-night motility distribution.

[1]  Marie J. Hayes,et al.  Spontaneous movements during sleep in children: temporal organization and changes with age. , 1998, Developmental psychobiology.

[2]  E. Marder,et al.  Principles of rhythmic motor pattern generation. , 1996, Physiological reviews.

[3]  Jacob Cohen A Coefficient of Agreement for Nominal Scales , 1960 .

[4]  M. Vecchierini-Blineau,et al.  Maturation de la motilité généralisée, spontanée au cours du sommeil, de la naissance à terme à l'âge de 6 mois , 1994, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.

[5]  P. Peirano,et al.  Influence of sleep state and age on body motility in normal premature and full-term neonates. , 1986, Neuropediatrics.

[6]  F. Bes,et al.  24-hour behavioural states distribution in continuously fed infants. , 1988, Early human development.

[7]  P. Eken,et al.  The spectrum of leukomalacia using cranial ultrasound , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[8]  P. Salzarulo,et al.  Sleep states development in the first year of life assessed through 24-h recordings. , 1982, Early human development.

[9]  W. P. Smotherman,et al.  Cyclic motor activity in the fetal rat (Rattus norvegicus). , 1988, Journal of Comparative Psychology.

[10]  I. McMillen,et al.  Development of Circadian Sleep-Wake Rhythms in Preterm and Full-Term Infants , 1991, Pediatric Research.

[11]  M. Sterman,et al.  Temporal distribution of sleep states, somatic activity, and autonomic activity during the first half year of life. , 1982, Sleep.

[12]  Marie J. Hayes,et al.  Functional analysis of spontaneous movements in preterm infants. , 1994, Developmental psychobiology.

[13]  Mills Jn Developement of circadian rhythms in infancy. , 1975 .

[14]  J. D. de Vries,et al.  The emergence of fetal behaviour. I. Qualitative aspects. , 1982, Early human development.

[15]  G. Cioni,et al.  Prognostic value of abnormal EEG transients in preterm and full-term neonates. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[16]  M. Mirmiran,et al.  Circadian rhythms in early human development. , 1991, Early human development.

[17]  G Cioni,et al.  Background EEG activity in preterm infants: correlation of outcome with selected maturational features. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[18]  G. Cioni,et al.  Preterm and early postterm motor behaviour in low-risk premature infants. , 1990, Early human development.

[19]  H. Prechtl,et al.  State of the art of a new functional assessment of the young nervous system. An early predictor of cerebral palsy. , 1997, Early human development.

[20]  P. Wolff Discontinuous changes in human wakefulness around the end of the second month of life: a developmental perspective , 1984 .

[21]  H. Prechtl,et al.  Developmental course of general movements in early infancy. I. Descriptive analysis of change in form. , 1992, Early human development.

[22]  N. Kudo,et al.  Spontaneous motoneuronal activity mediated by glycine and GABA in the spinal cord of rat fetuses in vitro. , 1996, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  G. Visser,et al.  The emergence of fetal behaviour. III. Individual differences and consistencies. , 1988, Early human development.

[24]  A. H. Parmelee,et al.  11 – Development of States in Infants* , 1972 .

[25]  A. Roberts,et al.  NORMAL PATTERNS OF FETAL ACTIVITY IN THE THIRD TRIMESTER , 1979 .

[26]  S. Robertson Temporal Organizaton in Fetal and Newborn Movement , 1990 .

[27]  G Cioni,et al.  Comparison between observation of spontaneous movements and neurologic examination in preterm infants. , 1997, The Journal of pediatrics.

[28]  E. Thoman,et al.  Sleep in premature and fullterm infants from 24-hour home recordings☆ , 1994 .

[29]  G. Visser,et al.  Diurnal and other variations in fetal movement and heart rate patterns at 20-22 weeks. , 1987, Early human development.

[30]  M. Hadders‐Algra,et al.  The effect of behavioural state on general movements in healthy full-term newborns. A polymyographic study. , 1993, Early human development.

[31]  J. L. Myers Fundamentals of Experimental Design , 1972 .

[32]  Giovanni Cioni,et al.  An early marker for neurological deficits after perinatal brain lesions , 1997, The Lancet.

[33]  L. S. James,et al.  A scoring system for states of sleep and wakefulness in term and preterm infants. , 1984, Pediatric research.

[34]  D. Arduini,et al.  modifications of ultradian and orcadian rhythms of fetal heart rate after fetal‐maternal adrenal gland suppression: A double blind study , 1986, Prenatal diagnosis.

[35]  B. Hopkins,et al.  Developmental transformations of spontaneous movements in early infancy. , 1986, Early human development.

[36]  P. Peirano,et al.  Development of sleep states in normal premature and full-term newborns. , 1988, Developmental psychobiology.

[37]  R. Ariagno,et al.  Biological rhythmicity in preterm infants prior to discharge from neonatal intensive care. , 1995, Pediatrics.

[38]  L. S. James,et al.  VARIABILITY IN 24 HOUR STATE DISTRIBUTION IN PRETERM INFANTS , 1984, Pediatric Research.