Practice Induces Function-Specific Changes in Brain Activity

Background Practice can have a profound effect on performance and brain activity, especially if a task can be automated. Tasks that allow for automatization typically involve repeated encoding of information that is paired with a constant response. Much remains unknown about the effects of practice on encoding and response selection in an automated task. Methodology To investigate function-specific effects of automatization we employed a variant of a Sternberg task with optimized separation of activity associated with encoding and response selection by means of m-sequences. This optimized randomized event-related design allows for model free measurement of BOLD signals over the course of practice. Brain activity was measured at six consecutive runs of practice and compared to brain activity in a novel task. Principal Findings Prompt reductions were found in the entire cortical network involved in encoding after a single run of practice. Changes in the network associated with response selection were less robust and were present only after the third run of practice. Conclusions/Significance This study shows that automatization causes heterogeneous decreases in brain activity across functional regions that do not strictly track performance improvement. This suggests that cognitive performance is supported by a dynamic allocation of multiple resources in a distributed network. Our findings may bear importance in understanding the role of automatization in complex cognitive performance, as increased encoding efficiency in early stages of practice possibly increases the capacity to otherwise interfering information.

[1]  D. Manoach Prefrontal cortex dysfunction during working memory performance in schizophrenia: reconciling discrepant findings , 2003, Schizophrenia Research.

[2]  J. Desmond,et al.  The neural basis of visual skill learning: an fMRI study of mirror reading. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  M. Manosevitz,et al.  High-Speed Scanning in Human Memory , 2022 .

[4]  M D'Esposito,et al.  The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: effects of memory load and individual differences. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  B. Rypma,et al.  Factors controlling neural activity during delayed-response task performance: Testing a memory organization hypothesis of prefrontal function , 2006, Neuroscience.

[6]  John E. Desmond,et al.  Temporal dynamics of cerebro-cerebellar network recruitment during a cognitive task , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  C H Lücking,et al.  Changes in cortical activation during mirror reading before and after training: an fMRI study of procedural learning. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[8]  Richard L. Lewis,et al.  The mind and brain of short-term memory. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[9]  Xingbao Li,et al.  FUNCTIONAL NEUROANATOMY OF SUBCOMPONENT COGNITIVE PROCESSES INVOLVED IN VERBAL WORKING MEMORY , 2005, The International journal of neuroscience.

[10]  A. Kelly,et al.  Human functional neuroimaging of brain changes associated with practice. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  J H Duyn,et al.  fMRI study of effort and information processing in a working memory task , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[12]  Douglas N Greve,et al.  Identifying regional activity associated with temporally separated components of working memory using event-related functional MRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[13]  J. Duyn,et al.  Design of a SENSE‐optimized high‐sensitivity MRI receive coil for brain imaging , 2002, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[14]  Tara A. Cairo,et al.  Functional connectivity reveals load dependent neural systems underlying encoding and maintenance in verbal working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.

[15]  Nick F. Ramsey,et al.  Functional Anatomical Correlates of Controlled and Automatic Processing , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  Renxin Chu,et al.  Scalable multichannel MRI data acquisition system , 2004, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[17]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  How does practice reduce dual-task interference: Integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening? , 2006, Psychological research.

[18]  P. Boesiger,et al.  SENSE: Sensitivity encoding for fast MRI , 1999, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[19]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Why practice reduces dual-task interference. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  From cognitive to neural models of working memory , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  Hugh Garavan,et al.  Practice‐related functional activation changes in a working memory task , 2000, Microscopy research and technique.

[22]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[23]  S. Petersen,et al.  Practice-related changes in human brain functional anatomy during nonmotor learning. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  John Jonides,et al.  How does practice makes perfect? , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  W. Schneider,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of practice-related change: fMRI and meta-analytic evidence of a domain-general control network for learning. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[26]  Renxin Chu,et al.  Magnetic Resonance in Medicine 51:22–26 (2004) Signal-to-Noise Ratio and Parallel Imaging Performance of a 16-Channel Receive-Only Brain Coil Array at , 2022 .

[27]  J. Duncan,et al.  Encoding Strategies Dissociate Prefrontal Activity from Working Memory Demand , 2003, Neuron.

[28]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Flexible cognitive control: Effects of individual differences and brief practice on a complex cognitive task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[29]  R. Passingham Attention to action. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[30]  N F Ramsey,et al.  Neurophysiological factors in human information processing capacity. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  Jacco A. de Zwart,et al.  Method for functional MRI mapping of nonlinear response , 2003, NeuroImage.

[32]  Todd S Woodward,et al.  The influence of working memory load on phase specific patterns of cortical activity. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[33]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[34]  R. Passingham,et al.  The Time Course of Changes during Motor Sequence Learning: A Whole-Brain fMRI Study , 1998, NeuroImage.

[35]  S. Petersen,et al.  The effects of practice on the functional anatomy of task performance. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  Adrian M Owen,et al.  A common prefrontal-parietal network for mnemonic and mathematical recoding strategies within working memory. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  J. Cummings,et al.  Frontal-subcortical circuits: the anatomic basis of executive, social and motivated behaviors , 1997, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[38]  B. Postle Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain , 2006, Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Manoach,et al.  Prefrontal cortex fMRI signal changes are correlated with working memory load , 1997, Neuroreport.

[40]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Dissociating the effects of Sternberg working memory demands in prefrontal cortex , 2007, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[41]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The Influence of Working-Memory Demand and Subject Performance on Prefrontal Cortical Activity , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  H Bruder,et al.  Image reconstruction for echo planar imaging with nonequidistant k‐space sampling , 1992, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[43]  Yaakov Stern,et al.  An event-related fMRI study of the neural networks underlying the encoding, maintenance, and retrieval phase in a delayed-match-to-sample task. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[44]  Heinrich Sauer,et al.  Temporal changes in neural activation during practice of information retrieval from short-term memory: An fMRI study , 2006, Brain Research.

[45]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  A functional MRI study of the influence of practice on component processes of working memory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[46]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Changes in cortical activity after training of working memory — a single-subject analysis , 2007, Physiology & Behavior.

[48]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .

[49]  John E. Desmond,et al.  Load- and practice-dependent increases in cerebro-cerebellar activation in verbal working memory: an fMRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[50]  B. Postle,et al.  Maintenance versus Manipulation of Information Held in Working Memory: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 1999, Brain and Cognition.