Time-critical integration of formants for perception of communication calls in mice

Brain mechanisms in humans group together acoustical frequency components both in the spectral and temporal domain, which leads to the perception of auditory objects and of streams of sound events that are of biological and communicative significance. At the perceptual level, behavioral data on mammals that clearly support the presence of common concepts for processing species-specific communication sounds are unavailable. Here, we synthesize 17 models of mouse pup wriggling calls, present them in sequences of four calls to the pups' mothers in a natural communication situation, and record the maternal response behavior. We show that the biological significance of a call sequence depends on grouping together three predominant frequency components (formants) to an acoustic object within a critical time window of about 30-ms lead or lag time of the first formant. Longer lead or lag times significantly reduce the maternal responsiveness. Central inhibition seems to be responsible for setting this time window, which is also found in numerous perceptual studies in humans. Further, a minimum of 100-ms simultaneous presence of the three formants is necessary for occurrence of response behavior. As in humans, onset-time asynchronies of formants and formant durations interact nonlinearly to influence the adequate perception of a stream of sounds. Together, these data point to common rules for time-critical spectral integration, perception of acoustical objects, and auditory streaming (perception of an acoustical Gestalt) in mice and humans.

[1]  Stewart H. Hulse,et al.  The comparative psychology of audition : perceiving complex sounds , 1989 .

[2]  R. S. Waters,et al.  Speech perception by rhesus monkeys: The voicing distinction in synthesized labial and velar stop consonants , 1976 .

[3]  S. H. Hulse,et al.  Auditory scene analysis by European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris): perceptual segregation of tone sequences. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[4]  J D Miller,et al.  Speech perception by the chinchilla: identification function for synthetic VOT stimuli. , 1978, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  A S Bregman,et al.  Streaming vs. fusion of sinusoidal components of complex tones , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  Michael B. Calford,et al.  Monaural inhibition in cat auditory cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Q. Summerfield,et al.  On the dissociation of spectral and temporal cues to the voicing distinction in initial stop consonants. , 1977, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  William C. Stebbins,et al.  The significant features of Japanese macaque coo sounds: a psychophysical study , 1988, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  N Suga,et al.  Facilitative responses to species-specific calls in cortical FM-FM neurons of the mustached bat. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[10]  S. Pinker,et al.  Auditory streaming and the building of timbre. , 1978, Canadian journal of psychology.

[11]  Günter Ehret,et al.  Schallsignale Der Hausmaus (Mus Musculus) , 1974 .

[12]  D. Margoliash,et al.  Temporal and harmonic combination-sensitive neurons in the zebra finch's HVc , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  P. Jen,et al.  Temporally patterned pulse trains affect duration tuning characteristics of bat inferior collicular neurons , 1999, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[14]  J. H. Casseday,et al.  Neural tuning to sound duration in the inferior colliculus of the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  A. Bregman,et al.  Effect of silence between tones on auditory stream segregation. , 1976, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  G. Ehret,et al.  Categorical perception of mouse-pup ultrasounds in the temporal domain , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  H. Scheich,et al.  Responsiveness of units in the auditory neostriatum of the guinea fowl (Numida meleagris) to species-specific calls and synthetic stimuli , 1979, Journal of comparative physiology.

[18]  A. Doupe Song- and Order-Selective Neurons in the Songbird Anterior Forebrain and their Emergence during Vocal Development , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Wenstrup,et al.  Delay-tuned neurons in the inferior colliculus of the mustached bat: implications for analyses of target distance. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  D. Pisoni Identification and discrimination of the relative onset time of two component tones: implications for voicing perception in stops. , 1977, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  G. Ehret,et al.  Low-frequency sound communication by mouse pups (Mus musculus): wriggling calls release maternal behaviour , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  C S Watson,et al.  Auditory psychophysics and perception. , 1996, Annual review of psychology.

[23]  C. Schreiner,et al.  Physiology and topography of neurons with multipeaked tuning curves in cat primary auditory cortex. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Günter Ehret,et al.  Ultrasound recognition in house mice: Key-Stimulus configuration and recognition mechanism , 1982, Journal of comparative physiology.

[25]  Jun Yan,et al.  The midbrain creates and the thalamus sharpens echo-delay tuning for the cortical representation of target-distance information in the mustached bat , 1996, Hearing Research.

[26]  H. Scheich,et al.  Processing of sound sequences in macaque auditory cortex: response enhancement. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  N. Sutherland,et al.  Grouping Frequency Components of Vowels: When is a Harmonic not a Harmonic? , 1984 .

[28]  A. Feng,et al.  Neural basis of sound pattern recognition in anurans , 1990, Progress in Neurobiology.

[29]  N. Suga,et al.  Multiple combination-sensitive neurons in the auditory cortex of the mustached bat , 2001, Hearing Research.

[30]  P. Jen,et al.  Bicuculline application affects discharge pattern and pulse-duration tuning characteristics of bat inferior collicular neurons , 1999, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[31]  J. H. Casseday,et al.  Neural tuning for sound duration: role of inhibitory mechanisms in the inferior colliculus. , 1994, Science.

[32]  N. Suga,et al.  Specificity of combination-sensitive neurons for processing of complex biosonar signals in auditory cortex of the mustached bat. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  J. D. Miller,et al.  Speech perception by the chinchilla: voiced-voiceless distinction in alveolar plosive consonants , 1975, Science.

[34]  H. Scheich,et al.  Neuronal discrimination of natural and synthetic vowels in field L of trained mynah birds , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  J. H. Casseday,et al.  Timing in the auditory system of the bat. , 1999, Annual review of physiology.

[36]  Günter Ehret,et al.  Mice and humans perceive multiharmonic communication sounds in the same way , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  A. Liberman,et al.  The discrimination of relative onset-time of the components of certain speech and nonspeech patterns. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.

[38]  C. Darwin Perceiving vowels in the presence of another sound: constraints on formant perception. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[39]  N Suga,et al.  Combination-sensitive neurons in the medial geniculate body of the mustached bat: encoding of target range information. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  N Suga,et al.  Syntax processing by auditory cortical neurons in the FM-FM area of the mustached bat Pteronotus parnellii. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  C. Schreiner,et al.  Time course of forward masking tuning curves in cat primary auditory cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  W G Paschal,et al.  Frequency organization of delay-sensitive neurons in the auditory cortex of the FM bat, Myotis lucifugus. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  A S Feng,et al.  Temporal coding in the frog auditory midbrain: the influence of duration and rise-fall time on the processing of complex amplitude-modulated stimuli. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Jeffrey J. Wenstrup,et al.  Combination-sensitive neurons in the inferior colliculus , 1995, Hearing Research.

[45]  C. Schreiner,et al.  Sequence sensitivity of neurons in cat primary auditory cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  J. Wenstrup,et al.  Frequency organization and responses to complex sounds in the medial geniculate body of the mustached bat. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Alexander V. Galazyuk,et al.  Encoding of sound duration by neurons in the auditory cortex of the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus , 1997, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[48]  M M Merzenich,et al.  Neural Mechanisms Underlying Temporal Integration, Segmentation, and Input Sequence Representation: Some Implications for the Origin of Learning Disabilities a , 1993, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[49]  Mitchell Steinschneider,et al.  Neural correlates of auditory stream segregation in primary auditory cortex of the awake monkey , 2001, Hearing Research.

[50]  D P Phillips,et al.  Detection of silent intervals between noises activating different perceptual channels: some properties of "central" auditory gap detection. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[51]  N. Suga,et al.  Combination-sensitive neurons in the primary auditory cortex of the mustached bat , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[52]  Richard R Fay,et al.  Auditory stream segregation in goldfish (Carassius auratus) , 1998, Hearing Research.

[53]  C. Darwin,et al.  Grouping in pitch perception: effects of onset asynchrony and ear of presentation of a mistuned component. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[54]  M M Merzenich,et al.  Representation of a species-specific vocalization in the primary auditory cortex of the common marmoset: temporal and spectral characteristics. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  Günter Ehret,et al.  Critical bands and filter characteristics in the ear of the housemouse (Mus musculus) , 1976, Biological Cybernetics.

[56]  C. Darwin,et al.  The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section a Human Experimental Psychology Perceptual Grouping of Speech Components Differing in Fundamental Frequency and Onset-time Perceptual Grouping of Speech Components Differing in Fundamental Frequency and Onset-time , 2022 .

[57]  Richard R. Fay,et al.  Spectral contrasts underlying auditory stream segregation in goldfish (Carassius auratus) , 2000, Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology.

[58]  C. Darwin,et al.  The integration of nonsimultaneous frequency components into a single virtual pitch. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[59]  A. Huggins,et al.  Just noticeable differences for segment duration in natural speech. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[60]  Albert S. Feng,et al.  Frequency selectivity in the anuran auditory midbrain: Single unit responses to single and multiple tone stimulation , 1982, Journal of comparative physiology.

[61]  T. Hashikawa,et al.  Temporal Integration and Duration Tuning in the Dorsal Zone of Cat Auditory Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  B. Grothe,et al.  Duration tuning in the mouse auditory midbrain. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  S. H. Hulse,et al.  Auditory scene analysis by songbirds: stream segregation of birdsong by European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). , 1997, Journal of comparative psychology.

[64]  N. Suga,et al.  Cortical neurons sensitive to combinations of information-bearing elements of biosonar signals in the mustache bat. , 1978, Science.