Implicit attention to negative social, in contrast to nonsocial, words in the Stroop task differs between individuals high and low in loneliness: Evidence from event-related brain microstates

Being on the social perimeter is not only sad, it is dangerous. Our evolutionary model of the effects of perceived social isolation (loneliness) on the brain as well as a growing body of behavioral research suggests that loneliness promotes short-term self-preservation, including an increased implicit vigilance for social, in contrast to nonsocial, threats. However, this hypothesis has not been tested previously in a neuroimaging study. We therefore used high density EEG and a social Stroop interference task to test the hypothesis that implicit attention to negative social, in contrast to nonsocial, Words in the Stroop task differs between individuals high versus low in loneliness and to investigate the brain dynamics of implicit processing for negative social (vs nonsocial) stimuli in lonely individuals, compared to nonlonely individuals (N = 70). The present study provides the first evidence that negative social stimuli are differentiated from negative nonsocial stimuli more quickly in the lonely than nonlonely brains. Given the timing of this differentiation in the brain and the fact that participants were performing a Stroop task, these results also suggest that these differences reflect implicit rather than explicit attentional differences between lonely and nonlonely individuals. Source estimates were performed for purposes of hypothesis generation regarding underlying neural mechanisms, and the results implicated the neural circuits reminiscent of orienting and executive control aspects of attention as contributing to these differences. Together, the results are in accord with the evolutionary model of loneliness.

[1]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Hemispheric Specialization Varies with EEG Brain Resting States and Phase of Menstrual Cycle , 2013, PloS one.

[2]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Toward a neurology of loneliness. , 2014, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[4]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Microstates in resting-state EEG: Current status and future directions , 2015, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[5]  Jean Decety,et al.  In the Eye of the Beholder: Individual Differences in Perceived Social Isolation Predict Regional Brain Activation to Social Stimuli , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Robin M. Weiss,et al.  Dynamic spatiotemporal brain analyses using high performance electrical neuroimaging: Theoretical framework and validation , 2014, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[7]  M Corbetta,et al.  Attentional modulation of neural processing of shape, color, and velocity in humans. , 1990, Science.

[8]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  L. Hawkley,et al.  Loneliness is associated with sleep fragmentation in a communal society. , 2011, Sleep.

[10]  Tyler J VanderWeele,et al.  A marginal structural model analysis for loneliness: implications for intervention trials and clinical practice. , 2011, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[11]  J. Schneider,et al.  Loneliness and risk of Alzheimer disease. , 2007, Archives of general psychiatry.

[12]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[13]  Angelo Gemignani,et al.  How stressful are 105 days of isolation? Sleep EEG patterns and tonic cortisol in healthy volunteers simulating manned flight to Mars. , 2014, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[14]  Stephanie Cacioppo,et al.  The neuroendocrinology of social isolation. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[15]  N. Christakis,et al.  Alone in the Crowd: The Structure and Spread of Loneliness in a Large Social Network , 2009 .

[16]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[17]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Understanding Actions of Others: The Electrodynamics of the Left and Right Hemispheres. A High-Density EEG Neuroimaging Study , 2010, PloS one.

[18]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Human Cognition. , 1999, News in physiological sciences : an international journal of physiology produced jointly by the International Union of Physiological Sciences and the American Physiological Society.

[19]  Richard M. Leahy,et al.  Brainstorm: A User-Friendly Application for MEG/EEG Analysis , 2011, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[20]  Richard M. Leahy,et al.  Electromagnetic brain mapping , 2001, IEEE Signal Process. Mag..

[21]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Investigations of the functional anatomy of attention using the stroop test , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  J. Zubek,et al.  Relative effects of prolonged social isolation and confinement: Behavioral and EEG changes. , 1969, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[23]  Louise C Hawkley,et al.  Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[24]  Théodore Papadopoulo,et al.  OpenMEEG: opensource software for quasistatic bioelectromagnetics , 2010, Biomedical engineering online.

[25]  Jean Decety,et al.  The speed of morality: a high-density electrical neuroimaging study. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Loneliness , 2015, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[27]  F. Perrin,et al.  Mapping of scalp potentials by surface spline interpolation. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[28]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Attention Enhances the Neural Processing of Relevant Features and Suppresses the Processing of Irrelevant Features in Humans: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of the Stroop Task , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  R. Snaith,et al.  The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale , 1983 .

[30]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  M. Kauppinen,et al.  Depressive symptoms in late life: a 10-year follow-up. , 2004, Archives of gerontology and geriatrics.

[32]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  Loneliness: Human Nature and the Need for Social Connection , 2008 .

[33]  L. Hawkley,et al.  A Behavioral Taxonomy of Loneliness in Humans and Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) , 2014, PloS one.

[34]  Shelly L. Gable,et al.  Approach and avoidance social motives and goals. , 2006, Journal of personality.

[35]  M. Fox The effects of short-term social and sensory isolation upon behavior, EEG and averaged evoked potentials in puppies , 1967 .

[36]  D. Louis Collins,et al.  Design and construction of a realistic digital brain phantom , 1998, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.

[37]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings , 1999 .

[38]  D. Russell UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. , 1996, Journal of personality assessment.

[39]  I. Couzin,et al.  Predatory Fish Select for Coordinated Collective Motion in Virtual Prey , 2012, Science.

[40]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Principles of spatial analysis , 1987 .

[41]  Olivier D. Faugeras,et al.  A common formalism for the Integral formulations of the forward EEG problem , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.

[42]  D. Chandler,et al.  The Effects of Environmental Enrichment on Depressive and Anxiety-Relevant Behaviors in Socially Isolated Prairie Voles , 2014, Psychosomatic medicine.

[43]  Munirah Bangee,et al.  Loneliness and attention to social threat in young adults: Findings from an eye tracker study , 2014 .

[44]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Segmentation of brain electrical activity into microstates: model estimation and validation , 1995, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[45]  L. Hawkley,et al.  Perceived social isolation and cognition , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[46]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  fMRI Studies of Stroop Tasks Reveal Unique Roles of Anterior and Posterior Brain Systems in Attentional Selection , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential fields. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[48]  Jean Decety,et al.  Unconscious affective processing and empathy: An investigation of subliminal priming on the detection of painful facial expressions , 2009, Pain.

[49]  Alexandra Barlow,et al.  Investigating Hypervigilance for Social Threat of Lonely Children , 2013, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[50]  E. Adam,et al.  Day-to-day dynamics of experience–cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[51]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[52]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  Loneliness within a nomological net: An evolutionary perspective , 2006 .

[53]  Erik Asp,et al.  A Quantitative Meta-Analysis of Functional Imaging Studies of Social Rejection , 2013, Scientific Reports.