The production of coerced expressions: Evidence from priming

[1]  T. Jaeger,et al.  Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.

[2]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  Participant role and syntactic alignment in dialogue , 2007 .

[3]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Referring as a collaborative process , 1986, Cognition.

[4]  Brian McElree,et al.  Complement Coercion Is Not Modulated by Competition: Evidence from Eye Movements , 2022 .

[5]  Alexandra A. Cleland,et al.  Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue , 2000, Cognition.

[6]  Ted Briscoe,et al.  Enjoy the Paper: Lexicology , 1990, COLING.

[7]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  Deferred Interpretations: Why Starting Dickens is Taxing but Reading Dickens Isn't , 2006, Cogn. Sci..

[8]  Alex Lascarides,et al.  Pragmatics and word meaning , 1998, Journal of Linguistics.

[9]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  An empirical investigation into spatial reference frame taxonomy using dialogue. , 2006 .

[10]  Zenzi M. Griffin,et al.  Conceptual structure modulates structural priming in the production of complex sentences. , 2003, Journal of memory and language.

[11]  Holly P. Branigan,et al.  Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Cantonese–Mandarin bilinguals , 2011 .

[12]  R. Krauss,et al.  Changes in reference phrases as a function of frequency of usage in social interaction: a preliminary study , 1964 .

[13]  Alexandra A. Cleland,et al.  The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure , 2003 .

[14]  Ray Jackendoff,et al.  The Architecture of the Language Faculty , 1996 .

[15]  M. Pickering,et al.  The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming , 2007 .

[16]  Ann Copestake,et al.  The semi-generative lexicon: limits on lexical productivity , 2001 .

[17]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  A time course analysis of enriched composition , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[18]  Frank Keller,et al.  Intra-sentential context effects on the interpretation of logical metonymy , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[19]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[20]  R. Baayen,et al.  Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .

[21]  M. Pickering,et al.  The Representation of Verbs: Evidence from Syntactic Priming in Language Production , 1998 .

[22]  S. Garrod,et al.  Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination , 1987, Cognition.

[23]  D. Barr,et al.  Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.

[24]  W. Levelt,et al.  Lexical access in the production of pronouns , 1999, Cognition.

[25]  Adele E. Goldberg,et al.  Can thematic roles leave traces of their places? , 2003, Cognition.

[26]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[27]  Ted Briscoe,et al.  Semi-productive Polysemy and Sense Extension , 1995, J. Semant..

[28]  M. Pickering,et al.  Structural priming: a critical review. , 2008, Psychological bulletin.

[29]  J. K. Bock Syntactic persistence in language production , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  Mirella Lapata,et al.  Evidence for serial coercion: A time course analysis using the visual-world paradigm , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  The difficulty of coercion: A response to de Almeida , 2005, Brain and Language.

[32]  Alexandra A. Cleland,et al.  Syntactic alignment and participant role in dialogue , 2007, Cognition.

[33]  K. Bock,et al.  From conceptual roles to structural relations: bridging the syntactic cleft. , 1992 .

[34]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  Context effects in coercion: Evidence from eye movements , 2005 .

[35]  F. Ferreira Choice of Passive Voice is Affected by Verb Type and Animacy , 1994 .

[36]  M. H. Kelly,et al.  Word and World Order: Semantic, Phonological, and Metrical Determinants of Serial Position , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[37]  Andrew J. Stewart,et al.  Syntactic priming: Investigating the mental representation of language , 1995 .

[38]  Andrea E. Martin,et al.  The Anterior Midline Field: coercion or decision making? , 2009 .

[39]  Holly P. Branigan,et al.  Mapping Concepts to Syntax: Evidence from Structural Priming in Mandarin Chinese. , 2012 .

[40]  Kathryn Bock,et al.  Language production : Grammatical encoding , 1994 .

[41]  G. Dell,et al.  Syllable Structure in Speech Production: Are Syllables Chunks or Schemas? , 1995 .

[42]  Anne Cutler,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[43]  Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al.  Effects of phonological feedback on the selection of syntax: Evidence from between-language syntactic priming* , 2011, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[44]  James Pustejovsky,et al.  The Generative Lexicon , 1995, CL.

[45]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[46]  J. K. Bock,et al.  Conceptual accessibility and syntactic structure in sentence formulation , 1985, Cognition.

[47]  James Pustejovsky,et al.  The syntax of event structure , 1991, Cognition.

[48]  H. H. Clark Responding to indirect speech acts , 1979, Cognitive Psychology.

[49]  Brian McElree,et al.  Reading time evidence for enriched composition , 2001, Cognition.

[50]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  Coercion in sentence processing: evidence from eye-movements and self-paced reading , 2002 .

[51]  Karl G. D. Bailey,et al.  Do speakers and listeners observe the Gricean Maxim of Quantity , 2006 .