Map-following skills in left and right brain-damaged patients with and without hemineglect

Map-following tasks require a “semantic interpretation” of the map, which could be affected by left brain damage, and “superimposition of the map upon the space,” which could be compromised by right lesions and particularly by the presence of hemineglect. Participants followed a pathway depicted on a map of a real environment. The pathway included four left and four right turns. A legend explained the meaning of each symbol that appeared on the map. Our results showed no deficits in left brain-damaged patients, but poor performance in right brain-damaged patients affected by hemineglect. This deficit can be ascribed to their impaired egocentric frame of reference, but we cannot exclude a prevalent role of the right hemisphere in their use of the allocentric information on the map despite the presence of hemineglect. Indeed, three right brain-damaged patients without hemineglect showed a specific deficit in performing the task. We discuss the results in light of the possible impairment of the parietomedial temporal pathway, which supports spatial navigation and could be responsible for the patients' deficit.

[1]  P. Garthwaite,et al.  Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  M. Conson,et al.  Categorical and coordinate spatial processing in the imagery domain investigated by rTMS , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  C. Caltagirone,et al.  The Mental Deterioration Battery: Normative Data, Diagnostic Reliability and Qualitative Analyses of Cognitive Impairment , 1996 .

[4]  H. Goodglass,et al.  Localization of body parts in brain injured subjects , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Cortical Modulation of Whole Body Movements in Brain-Damaged Patients , 2003, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[6]  A. Berthoz,et al.  The neural basis of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in humans: a functional magnetic resonance study , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  Luigi Pizzamiglio,et al.  Evidence for Separate Allocentric and Egocentric Space Processing in Neglect Patients , 1998, Cortex.

[8]  E Capitani,et al.  Raven's coloured progressive matrices: normative values on 305 adult normal controls. , 1987, Functional neurology.

[9]  I. Izquierdo,et al.  Both the dorsal hippocampus and the dorsolateral striatum are needed for rat navigation in the Morris water maze , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.

[10]  C. B. Cave,et al.  Evidence for two types of spatial representations: hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate relations. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Updating of locations during whole-body rotations in patients with hemispatial neglect , 2001, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[12]  J. C. Malinowski,et al.  Mental Rotation and Real-World Wayfinding , 2001, Perceptual and motor skills.

[13]  A. Kertesz Localization in neuropsychology , 1983 .

[14]  Marcella Laiacona,et al.  Tre test clinici di memoria verbale a lungo termine: Taratura su soggetti normali. , 1986 .

[15]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Assessment and treatment of childhood topographical disorientation: A case study , 2007, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.

[16]  G. Iaria,et al.  Representational neglect and navigation in real space , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  D. Perani,et al.  The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  G. Iaria,et al.  Mental imagery skills and topographical orientation in humans: A correlation study , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[19]  Carlo Caltagirone,et al.  Standardizzazione di due test di memoria per uso clinico: Breve Racconto e Figura di Rey , 2002 .

[20]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Improving Lesion-Symptom Mapping , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  D. C. Howell,et al.  Comparing an Individual's Test Score Against Norms Derived from Small Samples , 1998 .

[22]  Shannon E. McCarthy,et al.  Kindergarten children's sensitivity to geometry in maps. , 2011, Developmental science.

[23]  Clare Davies,et al.  Map use and the Development of Spatial Cognition , 2007 .

[24]  E. Mayer,et al.  A pure case of Gerstmann syndrome with a subangular lesion. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Laura Piccardi,et al.  Representational neglect and navigation in virtual space , 2009, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[26]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  More Is Not Always Better: Increased Fractional Anisotropy of Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus Associated with Poor Visuospatial Abilities in Williams Syndrome , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space , 1978, Cortex.

[28]  Pierre Pica,et al.  Core knowledge of geometry in an Amazonian indigene group. , 2006, Science.

[29]  L. Pelosi,et al.  On the different roles of the cerebral hemispheres in mental imagery: The “o'Clock Test” in two clinical cases , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[30]  F. Newcombe,et al.  Disorders of space exploration and cognition E. De Renzi. John Wiley, Chichester (1982). 268 pp. , 1982, Neuroscience.

[31]  G. Iaria,et al.  Lack of orientation due to a congenital brain malformation: A case study , 2005, Neurocase.

[32]  A. Postma,et al.  On the hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate spatial relations: a review of the current evidence , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  Anthony J. Aretz,et al.  The Mental Rotation of Map Displays , 1992 .

[34]  Liana Palermo,et al.  Hemispheric contribution to categorical and coordinate representational processes: A study on brain-damaged patients , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  M. L. Rusconi,et al.  Unilateral neglect in route description , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  R. Downs,et al.  Understanding maps as symbols: the development of map concepts in children. , 1989, Advances in child development and behavior.

[37]  L. Acredolo,et al.  Developmental Changes in Map-Reading Skills. , 1979 .

[38]  J. M. Blaut Environmental Mapping in Young Children , 1970 .

[39]  C. Kennard,et al.  Distractor-dependent frontal neglect , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  A fronto-parietal system for computing the egocentric spatial frame of reference in humans , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  D. Nico,et al.  Landmark based navigation in brain-damaged patients with neglect , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  J. Deloache Becoming symbol-minded , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .

[44]  Georg Kerkhoff,et al.  Spatial hemineglect in humans , 2001, Progress in Neurobiology.

[45]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Spatial representation across species: geometry, language, and maps , 2009, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[46]  P Bartolomeo,et al.  The relationship between visuospatial and representational neglect , 1994, Neurology.

[47]  Jason W. Brown Neuropsychology of Visual Perception , 1989 .

[48]  Dwight J. Kravitz,et al.  A new neural framework for visuospatial processing , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  M. Cherrier,et al.  Agnosia for scenes in topographagnosia , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  K. Heilman,et al.  Pure Gerstmann's syndrome from a focal lesion. , 1983, Archives of neurology.

[51]  A. Hof,et al.  Walking trajectory in neglect patients. , 2006, Gait & Posture.

[52]  R. Golledge Behavioral Geography and the Theoretical/Quantitative Revolution , 2008 .

[53]  Pierluigi Zoccolotti,et al.  Functional evaluation of hemineglect by means of a semistructured scale: Personal extrapersonal differentiation , 1991 .

[54]  L. Pizzamiglio,et al.  Toward a comprehensive diagnosis of visual-spatial disorders in unilateral brain damaged patients. , 1989 .

[55]  Lyndsey Nickels,et al.  Topographical disorientation: Towards an integrated framework for assessment , 2007, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.

[56]  P. Rubé,et al.  L’examen Clinique en Psychologie , 1959 .

[57]  Bruno B Averbeck,et al.  Understanding the parietal lobe syndrome from a neurophysiological and evolutionary perspective , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[58]  D. Pandya,et al.  Segmentation of subcomponents within the superior longitudinal fascicle in humans: a quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[59]  D. Benson,et al.  Loss of topographic familiarity. An environmental agnosia. , 1986, Archives of neurology.

[60]  B. Laeng Lateralization of Categorical and Coordinate Spatial Functions: A Study of Unilateral Stroke Patients , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  H. Karnath,et al.  Using human brain lesions to infer function: a relic from a past era in the fMRI age? , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[62]  Philip M. Corsi Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. , 1972 .

[63]  Laurel J Buxbaum,et al.  Assessment of spatial attention and neglect with a virtual wheelchair navigation task , 2008, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[64]  F. Dick,et al.  Voxel-based lesion–symptom mapping , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[65]  Marina Vasilyeva,et al.  Children's use of geometric information in mapping tasks. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[66]  Anna Shusterman,et al.  Young children's spontaneous use of geometry in maps. , 2008, Developmental science.

[67]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Neuroimaging Studies of Mental Rotation: A Meta-analysis and Review , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[68]  A. Yamadori,et al.  Pure Topographical Disorientation Related to Dysfunction of the Viewpoint Dependent Visual System , 1998, Cortex.

[69]  L. Rapport,et al.  Rightward orienting bias, wheelchair maneuvering, and fall risk. , 1995, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[70]  L. Rapport,et al.  Effect of attentional bias to right space on wheelchair mobility. , 1994, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[71]  Nicola Smania,et al.  Coding of far and near space during walking in neglect patients. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[72]  Federico Nemmi,et al.  Cognitive maps in imagery neglect , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[73]  P. Garthwaite,et al.  Point and interval estimates of effect sizes for the case-controls design in neuropsychology: Rationale, methods, implementations, and proposed reporting standards , 2010, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[74]  Raffaella Nori,et al.  Does hemineglect affect visual mental imagery? Imagery deficits in representational and perceptual neglect , 2010, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[75]  S. Moffat,et al.  Navigation in a “Virtual” Maze: Sex Differences and Correlation With Psychometric Measures of Spatial Ability in Humans , 1998 .

[76]  H. Teuber,et al.  Spatial Orientation in Man After Cerebral Injury: I. Analyses by Locus of Lesion , 1955 .

[77]  Pierluigi Zoccolotti,et al.  Psychometric Characteristics of Two Semi-structured Scales for the Functional Evaluation of Hemi- inattention in Extrapersonal and Personal Space , 1992 .

[78]  Amy Lobben,et al.  Tasks, Strategies, and Cognitive Processes Associated With Navigational Map Reading: A Review Perspective* , 2004, The Professional Geographer.