An assessment of auditory-guided locomotion in an obstacle circumvention task

This study investigated how effectively audition can be used to guide navigation around an obstacle. Ten blindfolded normally sighted participants navigated around a 0.6 × 2 m obstacle while producing self-generated mouth click sounds. Objective movement performance was measured using a Vicon motion capture system. Performance with full vision without generating sound was used as a baseline for comparison. The obstacle’s location was varied randomly from trial to trial: it was either straight ahead or 25 cm to the left or right relative to the participant. Although audition provided sufficient information to detect the obstacle and guide participants around it without collision in the majority of trials, buffer space (clearance between the shoulder and obstacle), overall movement times, and number of velocity corrections were significantly (p < 0.05) greater with auditory guidance than visual guidance. Collisions sometime occurred under auditory guidance, suggesting that audition did not always provide an accurate estimate of the space between the participant and obstacle. Unlike visual guidance, participants did not always walk around the side that afforded the most space during auditory guidance. Mean buffer space was 1.8 times higher under auditory than under visual guidance. Results suggest that sound can be used to generate buffer space when vision is unavailable, allowing navigation around an obstacle without collision in the majority of trials.

[1]  John M. Franchak,et al.  Perception of passage through openings depends on the size of the body in motion , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Franco Lepore,et al.  Early- and Late-Onset Blind Individuals Show Supra-Normal Auditory Abilities in Far-Space , 2004, Current Biology.

[3]  Andrew J. Kolarik,et al.  Sensory substitution information informs locomotor adjustments when walking through apertures , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  Lawrence D. Rosenblum,et al.  Echolocating Distance by Moving and Stationary Listeners , 2000 .

[5]  C. Richards,et al.  The negotiation of stationary and moving obstructions during walking: anticipatory locomotor adaptations and preservation of personal space. , 2005, Motor control.

[6]  Katja Fiehler,et al.  Neural Correlates of Human Echolocation of Path Direction During Walking. , 2015, Multisensory research.

[7]  J Douglas Crawford,et al.  Cortical mechanisms for trans-saccadic memory and integration of multiple object features , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  K. M. Dallenbach,et al.  Facial vision; perception of obstacles by the deaf-blind. , 1947, The American journal of psychology.

[9]  Jennifer L. Milne,et al.  Shape-specific activation of occipital cortex in an early blind echolocation expert , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Andrew J. Kolarik,et al.  Evidence for enhanced discrimination of virtual auditory distance among blind listeners using level and direct-to-reverberant cues , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  Mats E Nilsson,et al.  Human Echolocation: Blind and Sighted Persons' Ability to Detect Sounds Recorded in the Presence of a Reflecting Object , 2010, Perception.

[12]  Santani Teng,et al.  The acuity of echolocation: Spatial resolution in the sighted compared to expert performance. , 2011, Journal of visual impairment & blindness.

[13]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[14]  E. R. Strelow,et al.  Locomotion of the Blind Controlled by Natural Sound Cues , 1982, Perception.

[15]  M. Goodale,et al.  Citation for Published Item: Use Policy Neural Correlates of Natural Human Echolocation in Early and Late Blind Echolocation Experts , 2022 .

[16]  Pablo Luis López Espí,et al.  Physical Analysis of Several Organic Signals for Human Echolocation: Oral Vacuum Pulses , 2009 .

[17]  Daniel Rowan,et al.  Identification of the lateral position of a virtual object based on echoes by humans , 2013, Hearing Research.

[18]  David N. Lee,et al.  Common principle of guidance by echolocation and vision , 1992, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[19]  William R. Wiener,et al.  The Auditory Skills Necessary for Echolocation: A New Explanation , 1996 .

[20]  Giovanni Morone,et al.  Effects of Visual Deprivation on Gait Dynamic Stability , 2012, TheScientificWorldJournal.

[21]  Bradford J McFadyen,et al.  Characteristics of personal space during obstacle circumvention in physical and virtual environments. , 2008, Gait & posture.

[22]  Pavel Zahorik,et al.  Auditory distance perception in humans: a review of cues, development, neuronal bases, and effects of sensory loss , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[23]  David Guth,et al.  Echolocation Reconsidered: Using Spatial Variations in the Ambient Sound Field to Guide Locomotion , 1998 .

[24]  Victor Candas,et al.  Enhanced sensitivity to echo cues in blind subjects , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Kristoffer Hougaard Madsen,et al.  Neural correlates of virtual route recognition in congenital blindness , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  The role of head movements in the discrimination of 2-D shape by blind echolocation experts , 2014, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[27]  Lutz Wiegrebe,et al.  Ranging in Human Sonar: Effects of Additional Early Reflections and Exploratory Head Movements , 2014, PloS one.

[28]  Brian C. J. Moore,et al.  A summary of research investigating echolocation abilities of blind and sighted humans , 2014, Hearing Research.

[29]  K. M. Dallenbach,et al.  "Facial vision:" the role of pitch and loudness in the perception of obstacles by the blind. , 1950, The American journal of psychology.

[30]  David Whitney,et al.  Ultrafine spatial acuity of blind expert human echolocators , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Affordance-Based Control of Visually Guided Action , 2007 .

[32]  Jennifer L. Milne,et al.  Enhanced auditory spatial localization in blind echolocators , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  J. B. Pittenger,et al.  Human Echolocation as a Basic Form of Perception and Action , 1995 .

[34]  K. M. Dallenbach,et al.  Facial vision: the perception of obstacles out of doors by blindfolded and blindfolded-deafened subjects. , 1953, The American journal of psychology.

[35]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Absolute travel distance from optic flow , 2005, Vision Research.

[37]  T. Higuchi,et al.  Action‐oriented representation of peripersonal and extrapersonal space: Insights from manual and locomotor actions1 , 2006 .

[38]  P. Terrier,et al.  Role of visual input in the control of dynamic balance: variability and instability of gait in treadmill walking while blindfolded , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  A Straube,et al.  Visual stabilization of posture. Physiological stimulus characteristics and clinical aspects. , 1984, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  P. Worchel,et al.  The perception of obstacles by the blind. , 1950, Journal of experimental psychology.

[41]  Michael S. Gordon,et al.  Perception of Sound-Obstructing Surfaces Using Body-Scaled Judgments , 2004 .

[42]  Michael E Cinelli,et al.  Direction of single obstacle circumvention in middle-aged children. , 2014, Gait & posture.

[43]  Lutz Wiegrebe,et al.  Self-motion facilitates echo-acoustic orientation in humans , 2014, Royal Society Open Science.

[44]  Kathleen A. Turano,et al.  Optic-flow and egocentric-direction strategies in walking: Central vs peripheral visual field , 2005, Vision Research.

[45]  A. Bronkhorst,et al.  Auditory distance perception in humans : A summary of past and present research , 2005 .

[46]  W Schiff,et al.  Accuracy of judging time to arrival: effects of modality, trajectory, and gender. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  Lutz Wiegrebe,et al.  Discovering Your Inner Bat: Echo–Acoustic Target Ranging in Humans , 2012, Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology.

[48]  P. Aerts,et al.  Low vision affects dynamic stability of gait. , 2010, Gait & posture.

[49]  M. Paré,et al.  Early-blind human subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects , 1998, Nature.

[50]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Behavioral dynamics of steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  J. Lewald,et al.  Ventral and dorsal visual pathways support auditory motion processing in the blind: evidence from electrical neuroimaging , 2013, The European journal of neuroscience.

[52]  K. M. Dallenbach,et al.  "Facial Vision": The Perception of Obstacles by the Blind , 1944 .

[53]  P. Worchel,et al.  The effect of practice on the perception of obstacles by the blind. , 1951, Journal of experimental psychology.

[54]  Helen Pritchard,et al.  The effect of practice on the perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion. , 2022 .

[55]  J. Gibson Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. , 1998, British journal of psychology.

[56]  William H. Warren,et al.  Visually Controlled Locomotion: 40 years Later , 1998 .

[57]  D H Ashmead,et al.  Obstacle perception by congenitally blind children. , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[58]  Jörg Lewald,et al.  Exceptional ability of blind humans to hear sound motion: Implications for the emergence of auditory space , 2013, Neuropsychologia.