Developmental differences in the processing of afferent information for motor control

Chicoine, Lassonde, and Proteau (1992) recently proposed that young children processed independently from each other the different sources of sensory information available for controlling an aiming movement, but that maturation would result in the different sources of afferent information being processed intermodally. In this study we tested an alternative interpretation to that of Chicoine et al. Specifically, because of the limited time in which the participants of their study had to complete their movement, their results might indicate that the younger participants used an open‐looped mode of control. To test this possibility, children of different ages, as well as adults, practiced a complex, multisegment, aiming task in full vision prior to being submitted to a no‐vision transfer condition. Participants of all age‐groups had to complete the task in either 500 ms or 1,000 ms. Results indicated that the withdrawal of visual information in transfer caused larger aiming error for the adults than the youn...

[1]  Daniel Bullock,et al.  Neural dynamics of planned arm movements: emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation , 1988 .

[2]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .

[3]  G. Geffen,et al.  The development of tactile transfer of information , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  L. Hay,et al.  Visual Correction of a Rapid Goal-Directed Response , 1986, Perceptual and motor skills.

[5]  C. Bard,et al.  Role of peripheral vision in the directional control of rapid aiming movements , 1985 .

[6]  M. Lassonde,et al.  Absence of disconnexion syndrome in callosal agenesis and early callosotomy: Brain reorganization or lack of structural specificity during ontogeny? , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  D. Galin,et al.  Development of crossed and uncrossed tactile localization on the fingers , 1977, Brain and Language.

[8]  H. Birch,et al.  Visual differentiation, intersensory integration, and voluntary motor control. , 1967, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[9]  Chantal Bard,et al.  Etude ontogénétique de la coordination visuo-manuelle , 1983 .

[10]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  Adaptive neural networks for control of movement trajectories invariant under speed and force rescaling , 1991 .

[11]  L. Proteau Chapter 4 On The Specificity of Learning and the Role of Visual Information for Movement Control , 1992 .

[12]  C. Bard,et al.  Positional versus directional control loops in visuomotor pointing , 1990 .

[13]  H. Birch,et al.  INTERSENSORY DEVELOPMENT IN CHILDREN. , 1963, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[14]  Luc Proteau,et al.  Developmental aspects of sensorimotor integration , 1992 .

[15]  C. Hofsten,et al.  The integration of sensory information in the development of precise manual pointing , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  D. O'Leary A developmental study of interhemispheric transfer in children aged five to ten. , 1980, Child development.

[18]  R. Schmidt A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. , 1975 .

[19]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Control of multimovement coordination: sensorimotor mechanisms in speech motor programming. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[20]  D. Galin,et al.  Development of the capacity for tactile information transfer between hemispheres in normal children. , 1979, Science.

[21]  J. Paillard,et al.  Static versus Kinetic Visual Cues for the Processing of Spatial Relationships , 1985 .

[22]  K. Connolly,et al.  A developmental study of afferent-reafferent integration. , 1970, British journal of psychology.

[23]  J J Denier van der Gon,et al.  Mechanisms underlying accuracy in fast goal-directed arm movements in man. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.