Bilateral impairment of concurrent saccade programming in hemispatial neglect

When healthy observers make a saccade that is erroneously directed toward a distracter stimulus, they often produce a corrective saccade within 100ms after the end of the primary saccade. Such short inter-saccadic intervals indicate that programming of the secondary saccade has been initiated prior to the execution of the primary saccade and hence that the two saccades have been programmed concurrently. Here we show that concurrent saccade programming is bilaterally impaired in left spatial neglect, a strongly lateralized disorder of visual attention resulting from extensive right cerebral damage. Neglect patients were asked to make saccades to targets presented left or right of fixation while disregarding a distracter presented in the opposite hemifield. We examined those experimental trials on which participants first made a saccade to the distracter, followed by a secondary (corrective) saccade to the target. Compared to healthy and right-hemisphere damaged control participants the proportion of secondary saccades directing gaze to the target instead of bringing it even closer to the distracter was bilaterally reduced in neglect patients. In addition, the characteristic reduction of secondary saccade latency observed in both control groups was absent in neglect patients, whether the secondary saccade was directed to the left or right hemifield. This pattern is consistent with a severe, bilateral impairment of concurrent saccade programming in left spatial neglect.

[1]  I. Gilchrist,et al.  Manual responses and saccades in chronic and recovered hemispatial neglect: a study using visual search , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  G. Avanzini,et al.  Oculomotor disorders in cortical lesions in man: The role of unilateral neglect , 1983, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Sylvie Chokron,et al.  Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[4]  R. Walker,et al.  Saccadic eye movement programming in unilateral neglect , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Effects of similarity and history on neural mechanisms of visual selection , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[7]  Hiroshi Tsukagoshi,et al.  Eye-fixation patterns in homonymous hemianopia and unilateral spatial neglect , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Claus C Hilgetag,et al.  Functional circuitry underlying visual neglect. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  C. Kennard,et al.  Abnormal temporal dynamics of visual attention in spatial neglect patients , 1997, Nature.

[10]  W. Heide,et al.  Cortical control of double‐step saccades: Implications for spatial orientation , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[11]  R Tissot,et al.  Qualitative analysis of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to laterality of cerebral lesions , 1972, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[12]  W. Heide,et al.  Combined deficits of saccades and visuo-spatial orientation after cortical lesions , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  R. Remington Attention and saccadic eye movements. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  J. Findlay,et al.  The Relationship between Eye Movements and Spatial Attention , 1986, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[15]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search , 2000, Vision Research.

[16]  C. Rorden,et al.  Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. , 2000, Behavioural neurology.

[17]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[18]  H. Karnath,et al.  Trunk orientation as the determining factor of the 'contralateral' deficit in the neglect syndrome and as the physical anchor of the internal representation of body orientation in space. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  Visual Search Performance in the Neglect Syndrome , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  A. Young,et al.  Disentangling neglect and hemianopia , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Space exploration in neglect. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Superior colliculus activity related to concurrent processing of saccade goals in a visual search task. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[26]  M. Husain,et al.  Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Attention in Visual Neglect: A Case Study , 2004, Cortex.

[27]  J. Hornak,et al.  Ocular exploration in the dark by patients with visual neglect , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  L. Gauthier,et al.  The Bells Test: A quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. , 1989 .

[29]  E. Ldavas,et al.  Eye Movements and Orienting of Attention in Patients with Visual Neglect , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  René Müri,et al.  Cortical control of saccades , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[31]  H. Karnath,et al.  Exploratory saccades show no direction-specific deficit in neglect , 2000, Neurology.

[32]  Pascal Wurtz,et al.  One-Hertz transcranial magnetic stimulation over the frontal eye field induces lasting inhibition of saccade triggering , 2006, Neuroreport.

[33]  J. Sprague,et al.  Interaction of Cortex and Superior Colliculus in Mediation of Visually Guided Behavior in the Cat , 1966, Science.

[34]  R. Ptak,et al.  A non-spatial bias favouring fixated stimuli revealed in patients with spatial neglect. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[35]  M. Perenin,et al.  Tactile exploration of peripersonal space in patients with neglect , 1998, Neuroreport.

[36]  Aditya Murthy,et al.  Frontal eye field contributions to rapid corrective saccades. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  B. L. Zuber,et al.  Saccadic suppression: elevation of visual threshold associated with saccadic eye movements. , 1966, Experimental neurology.

[38]  C. Marzi,et al.  The spatial distribution of visual attention in hemineglect and extinction patients. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  T. Schenkenberg,et al.  Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment , 1980, Neurology.

[40]  Armin Schnider,et al.  Reflexive orienting in spatial neglect is biased towards behaviourally salient stimuli. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  Ian H. Robertson,et al.  Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies edited by Ian H. Robertson and John C. Marshall , 1994 .

[44]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  Searching with Unilateral Neglect , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  R. Klein,et al.  A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal lobe damage , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[46]  R. Müri,et al.  Visual exploration pattern in hemineglect , 2009, Psychological research.

[47]  R. Ptak,et al.  Temporal dynamics of attentional control settings in patients with spatial neglect , 2006, Brain Research.

[48]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  S. E. Black,et al.  Impaired visual search in patients with unilateral neglect: an oculographic analysis , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  R. Ptak,et al.  Looking left with left neglect: The role of spatial attention when active vision selects local image features for fixation , 2009, Cortex.

[51]  T. Paus,et al.  Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of the Human Frontal Eye Field: Effects on Visual Perception and Attention , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Properties of attentional selection during the preparation of sequential saccades , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[54]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Impairments of movement execution in unilateral neglect: A kinematic analysis of directional bradyskinesia , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  Radek Ptak,et al.  The Inferior Temporal Lobe Mediates Distracter-Resistant Visual Search of Patients with Spatial Neglect , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.