Systematic Biases in Human Heading Estimation

Heading estimation is vital to everyday navigation and locomotion. Despite extensive behavioral and physiological research on both visual and vestibular heading estimation over more than two decades, the accuracy of heading estimation has not yet been systematically evaluated. Therefore human visual and vestibular heading estimation was assessed in the horizontal plane using a motion platform and stereo visual display. Heading angle was overestimated during forward movements and underestimated during backward movements in response to both visual and vestibular stimuli, indicating an overall multimodal bias toward lateral directions. Lateral biases are consistent with the overrepresentation of lateral preferred directions observed in neural populations that carry visual and vestibular heading information, including MSTd and otolith afferent populations. Due to this overrepresentation, population vector decoding yields patterns of bias remarkably similar to those observed behaviorally. Lateral biases are inconsistent with standard Bayesian accounts which predict that estimates should be biased toward the most common straight forward heading direction. Nevertheless, lateral biases may be functionally relevant. They effectively constitute a perceptual scale expansion around straight ahead which could allow for more precise estimation and provide a high gain feedback signal to facilitate maintenance of straight-forward heading during everyday navigation and locomotion.

[1]  E. Peli,et al.  Heading Perception in Patients with Advanced Retinitis Pigmentosa , 2002, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[2]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Convergence of Vestibular and Visual Self-Motion Signals in an Area of the Posterior Sylvian Fissure , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Laura Telford,et al.  Role of optical flow field asymmetry in the perception of heading during linear motion , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  Roberta L Klatzky,et al.  Spatial updating of locations specified by 3-d sound and spatial language. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  R. Hetherington The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .

[6]  M. McCourt,et al.  Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  D. Kersten,et al.  Evidence in Human Subjects for Independent Coding of Azimuth and Elevation for Direction of Heading from Optic Flow , 1996, Vision Research.

[8]  François Klam,et al.  ã Federation of European Neuroscience Societies Visual±vestibular interactive responses in the macaque ventral intraparietal area (VIP) , 2022 .

[9]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Facts on optic flow , 1987, Biological Cybernetics.

[10]  W. Warren,et al.  Perception of translational heading from optical flow. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  Marianne Dieterich,et al.  Spatial neglect--a vestibular disorder? , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  John W Philbeck,et al.  Misperception of exocentric directions in auditory space. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[13]  Christopher R Fetsch,et al.  Neural correlates of reliability-based cue weighting during multisensory integration , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Estimating distance during self-motion: a role for visual-vestibular interactions. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[15]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Computational approaches to spatial orientation: from transfer functions to dynamic Bayesian inference. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Optic Flow Processing in Monkey STS: A Theoretical and Experimental Approach , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Frank H Durgin,et al.  Perceptual scale expansion: an efficient angular coding strategy for locomotor space , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[18]  W P Medendorp,et al.  Shared computational mechanism for tilt compensation accounts for biased verticality percepts in motion and pattern vision. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Felix Wichmann,et al.  The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  W. Warren,et al.  The role of central and peripheral vision in postural control duringwalking , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  T. Sanger,et al.  Probability density estimation for the interpretation of neural population codes. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Bayesian integration of visual and vestibular signals for heading. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[23]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. III. Response dynamics. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  M. De Vrijer,et al.  Accuracy-precision trade-off in visual orientation constancy. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[25]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Cardinal rules: Visual orientation perception reflects knowledge of environmental statistics , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[27]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[28]  Brian J. Fischer,et al.  Owl's behavior and neural representation predicted by Bayesian inference , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  F. Durgin,et al.  The perception of 2D orientation is categorically biased. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[30]  Yong Gu,et al.  Decoding of MSTd Population Activity Accounts for Variations in the Precision of Heading Perception , 2010, Neuron.

[31]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Encoding, learning, and spatial updating of multiple object locations specified by 3-D sound, spatial language, and vision , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  A Bayesian model of the disambiguation of gravitoinertial force by visual cues , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  M. Banks,et al.  Perceiving heading with different retinal regions and types of optic flow , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  Christopher R Fetsch,et al.  Dynamic Reweighting of Visual and Vestibular Cues during Self-Motion Perception , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  Naomi H. Feldman,et al.  The influence of categories on perception: explaining the perceptual magnet effect as optimal statistical inference. , 2009, Psychological review.

[37]  Gregory C. DeAngelis,et al.  Vestibular Facilitation of Optic Flow Parsing , 2010, PloS one.

[38]  S. Appelle Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.

[39]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Vestibular Heading Discrimination and Sensitivity to Linear Acceleration in Head and World Coordinates , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signals , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  C. Duffy MST neurons respond to optic flow and translational movement. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  M. Braunstein The role of central and peripheral vision in postural control duringwalking , 2010 .

[43]  Masao Ohmi,et al.  Heading judgments during active and passive self-motion , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  W. Warren,et al.  The role of central and peripheral vision in perceiving the direction of self-motion , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. I. Response to static tilts and to long-duration centrifugal force. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.