Colour contrast influences perceived shape in combined shading and texture patterns.

The 'colour-shading effect' describes the phenomenon whereby a chromatic pattern influences perceived shape-from-shading in a luminance pattern. Specifically, the depth corrugations perceived in sinusoidal luminance gratings can be enhanced by spatially non-aligned, and suppressed by spatially aligned sinusoidal chromatic gratings. Here we examine whether colour contrast can influence perceived shape in patterns that combine shape-from-shading with shape-from-texture. Stimuli consisted of sinusoidal modulations of texture (defined by orientation), luminance and colour. When the texture and luminance modulations were suitably combined, one obtained a vivid impression of a corrugated depth surface. The addition of a colour grating to the texture-luminance combination was found to enhance the impression of depth when out-of-phase with the luminance modulation, and suppress the impression of depth when in-phase with the luminance modulation. The degree of depth enhancement and depth suppression was approximately constant across texture amplitude when measured linearly. In the absence of the luminance grating however, the colour grating had no phase-dependent affect on perceived depth. These results show that colour contrast modulates the contribution of shading to perceived shape in combined shading and texture patterns.

[1]  Frederick A A Kingdom,et al.  Color brings relief to human vision , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  Frederick A.A. Kingdom,et al.  Chromatic properties of the colour-shading effect , 2005, Vision Research.

[3]  D. Fender,et al.  The interaction of color and luminance in stereoscopic vision. , 1972, Investigative ophthalmology.

[4]  Andrea Li,et al.  Perception of three-dimensional shape from texture is based on patterns of oriented energy , 2000, Vision Research.

[5]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Network model of shape-from-shading: neural function arises from both receptive and projective fields , 1988, Nature.

[6]  Pascal Mamassian,et al.  Interaction of visual prior constraints , 2001, Vision Research.

[7]  J. Rieger,et al.  Sensory and cognitive contributions of color to the recognition of natural scenes , 2000, Current Biology.

[8]  D. Knill Contour into texture: information content of surface contours and texture flow. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[9]  N. Dominy,et al.  Ecological importance of trichromatic vision to primates , 2001, Nature.

[10]  J. M. Rubin,et al.  Color vision and image intensities: When are changes material? , 1982, Biological Cybernetics.

[11]  Paul A. Griffin,et al.  Statistical Approach to Shape from Shading: Reconstruction of Three-Dimensional Face Surfaces from Single Two-Dimensional Images , 1996, Neural Computation.

[12]  J D Mollon,et al.  Catarrhine photopigments are optimized for detecting targets against a foliage background. , 2000, The Journal of experimental biology.

[13]  J. Mollon "Tho' she kneel'd in that place where they grew..." The uses and origins of primate colour vision. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[14]  J Sun,et al.  Shading and Stereo in Early Perception of Shape and Reflectance , 1997, Perception.

[15]  Frederick A A Kingdom,et al.  Colour Vision Brings Clarity to Shadows , 2004, Perception.

[16]  Kemt A. Stevens The Line of Curvature Constraint and the Interpretation of 3-D Shape From Parallel Surface Contours , 1983, IJCAI.

[17]  V. S. Ramachandran,et al.  Perception of shape from shading , 1988, Nature.

[18]  M. Landy,et al.  Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.

[19]  DH Hubel,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.